weblogUpdates.ping Taneak Jang, Rejang Land, Tanah Rejang http://rejang-lebong.blogspot.com Taneak Jang, Rejang land, Tanah Rejang
·

What The Rawas Dialect Reveals About The Linguistic History of Rejang

”The dialect which is probably most important from a historical point of view ... is the Jang Abeus dialect, spoken in the upper reaches of the river Rawas....In 1941 it still had the final -l in such words as biyol, Lebong biyoa water.”

--P. Voorhoeve, personal communication reported in Blust 1984:448, n.2.

1.0 Introduction[1]

Rejang Historical Phonology began with a pioneering article by Robert A. Blust (1984), in which contemporary Musi dialect data was derived from PMP via a set of (mostly regular) sound changes.[2] McGinn (1997) added new dialect data from the Kebanagung, Pesisir, and Lebong dialects in the attempt to explain some of the reported irregularities in the development of the vowels. McGinn (1999, 2000) explored some possible external subgrouping relationships for Rejang. Finally, although McGinn (2003) presented a reconstructed Proto-Rejang, owing to space limitations, only Rawas evidence was included. This paper attempts to fill some gaps in the record by displaying evidence from five major dialects against which the reconstructions can be tested and earlier work on the language can be verified or revised.

Rejang should be of interest to linguists for at least three reasons. (i) The position of Rejang as a linguistic isolate raises questions about the origin, migration route, and closest linguistic affiliations of the group (McGinn, 2003). (ii) Rejang exhibits more changes in the vowels than any other known Austronesian language: 27 splits of the original four PMP vowels are reflected in the Musi dialect, and 21 mergers (Blust 1984; McGinn 1997). (iii) Some apparently irregular vocalic developments affected pronouns and function words not only in Musi, but in all previously reported dialects, presenting prima facie evidence for recognition of non-phonetic conditions in the theory of sound change, refuting the neogrammarian position.

McGinn (1997) attempted to explain the irregular Musi function words in terms the placement of the reconstructed pre-Rejang accent (=word-level stress pattern), which fell on the penultimate vowel in pre-Rejang (whereas in all contemporary dialects the accent falls on the final syllabic vowel). The consequences of this analysis will be explored in relation to the contribution of Rawas to the historical phonology of Rejang, since in Rawas pronouns and other function words underwent the same regular changes as the content words.

More generally, it is fair to say that without Rawas, the dialects differ too little among themselves to offer much in the way of time depth, precluding any ambitions about reconstructing Proto-Rejang, not to mention establishing an external subgroup smaller than Malayo-Polynesian. With the addition of Rawas dialect evidence the picture has changed dramatically. The time depth has increased; the relationships among the dialects have begun to gain some clarity; the reconstruction of Proto-Rejang has become feasible; and some of the evidence pertaining to possible subgrouping relationships with other Austronesian languages has become clarified.

1.1 Location and Number of Rejang Speakers

When Richard Noss (1969) conducted a survey of language use in the late 1960s there were 204,000 Rejang speakers living in Bengkulu and South Sumatra. Noss’ estimate is consistent with Siddik (1980), but not Wurm & Hattori (1981), who give a much higher figure (one million). The higher number can perhaps be reconciled with the population of Bengkulu Province as a whole, including the city of Bengkulu with its majority Malay population, as well as the Kerinci, Serawai, Minangkabau, and transmigrated Javanese living in and around Rejang country.

1.2 Dialect Diversity

The following pair-wise cognate percentages were derived from data obtained in the respective dialect areas from bilingual speakers (Rejang and Indonesian) based on a standardized list of 200 Indonesian sentences prepared by Amran Halim (no date) to elicit the Swadesh 200-word list.

Table 1. Percentages of Shared Homosemantic CognatesA

Rejang Dialects

Pesisir

Lebong

Musi

Kebanagung

Rawas

Pesisir

XXXXX

88%

87.5%

75.5%

70.0%

Lebong

XXXXX

XXXXX

87.5%

78.0%

70.5%

Musi

XXXXX

XXXXX

XXXXX

82.0%

71.5%

Kebanagung

XXXXX

XXXXX

XXXXX

XXXXX

71.5%

Rawas

XXXXX

XXXXX

XXXXX

XXXXX

XXXXX

a Cognate percentages reported in McGinn (1983) are 5% to 10% higher than those

presented here. At that time I had access to dialect speakers living in the capital city,

Curup. The data in this paper were elicited from older speakers based on field work in

each dialect area.


Map of Rejang Country


The degree of variation among the dialects reported in McGinn (1983, 1997) was too slight to impede mutual understanding. By contrast, the Rawas dialect is incomprehensible to other Rejangs; I have played a recorded Rawas texts for Musi and Lebong speakers, always with the same result: they did not understand it. Vocabulary changes represent only part of the explanation, however. Many linguistic facts set Rawas apart from the other Rejang dialects. For instance, only Rawas retains the PMP diphthongs *iw and *uy as iw, uy; Rawas has a system of seven vowels (including low front ä), whereas the other dialects have six; Rawas reflects PMP *-j as -t in contrast to -g or -k in the other dialects; Rawas retains -l (derived from PMP *-R and *-l) as suggested by P. Voorhoeve in the quotation above; and (also as mentioned earlier) Rawas pronouns and other function words underwent the expected regular changes affecting word-final vowels, e.g. Rawas k«w 'I' and kum«w 'you (honorific)' alongside uku, kumu in the other dialects. The general impression is that Rawas is not only the most divergent among the five dialects, but also (pronouns notwithstanding) the most conservative. The remainder of this paper will substantiate this impression concerning the central importance of Rawas in determining the linguistic history of the Rejang language.

1.3. Synchronic Sketch of the Five Dialects

1.3.1 Contemporary Consonant Systems

Table 2. Contemporary Consonant Systems

Pesisir, Lebong, Musi Kebanagung Rawas

Stops & Affri- p t c k ? p t c k p t c k ?

cates b d j g b d j g b d j g

Fricatives s s h s h

Plain Nasals m n ñ N m n ñ N m n ñ N

Barred Nasals m n ñ N m n ñ N m n ñ N

Liquid l l l

Semivowels w y w y w y

Secondary phonemes and phoneme sequences mark loan words (usually from Malay). For example sergap 'attack' displays an instance of r and a sequence of two consonants within a morpheme; neither occurs in native Rejang words.

1.3.2 Contemporary Vowel Systems

Most dialects have six vowels (a, e, i, o, u, «); Rawas has a seventh vowel ä which regularly corresponds to e or ia or ea in the other dialects. See 2.4. Rawas ä is a low front vowel. The ä - e contrast was very clear to this observer, whereas the contrast between ä and a was very difficult to hear; mostly ä sounded like a; and either phoneme could manifest as low central [ Ã] in rapid speech. Several Malay-speaking onlookers in Surulangun were as puzzled as I when Pak Daud insisted that pät ‘bitter’ and pat ‘four' differed significantly in pronunciation. However, a (never ä) also varies freely with low back [] in certain phonetic environments. Phonologically all doubts disappear: Rawas pät from PMP *paqit ‘bitter’ contrasts with pat from PMP *epat `four’; and Rawas bäläk from PMP *balik `return home’ contrasts with balak `accident; occurrence’ (source unknown). Also important for what follows is the contrast between Rawas ä and e, although minimal pairs were not found: e.g. Rawas keke? `bad person’ vs. patäh `broken'; and kämäy '1Ppl(excl.)' vs. ete? `small’.

A seven-vowel system as witnessed in Rawas has been reconstructed for PR. See Table 4 below.

1.3.3. Contemporary Diphthongs and Accentuation

Although it is not always obvious, in all Rejang dialects the accent falls upon the final syllabic vowel of the word. The placement of the accent depends on syllable structure, and in particular on the prior identification of syllabic and non-syllabic vowels. See section 4.

Some consequences of syllabification and stress placement rules are as follows. First, morphemes like d«w [d«u9] ‘many’ and oa? [?oa9?] ‘far’ are monosyllabic and hence in metrical terms bear no word-level accent: instead, the observed contrast is between a vowel and a semivowel. Second, morphemes like um«a? [?um«:a9?] ‘home’ and tidoa [tido:a9] ‘sleep’ are disyllabic, with the accent falling on the second (final) syllabic vowel, as expected. Finally, canonical CVCVC morphs receive the accent on the final syllable as expected, e.g. Musi mono? [mono:?] `chicken’, s«lon [s«lo:n] `claw’, taN«n [taN«:n] 'hand'.

The complete set of Rejang diphthongs appears in Table 5 (from PMP diphthongs) and Table 13 (from PMP vowels).

1.4 Lexical-Phonological Conditions in Rejang Historical Phonology

As noted originally by Blust (1984), most vocalic changes in Rejang must be defined in terms of what he called ‘PMP sequences’; these are disyllabic (foot-length) and partially articulated speech segments such as *-aCi, *-aCiC, *-uCeC and so on. In this paper, these sequences are expanded in number and in quality (to include also the reconstructed accent) and labeled Gestalt Conditions (GCs). Two considerations make it necessary to find a replacement for Blust's term "PMP sequences". (i) Whereas PMP sequences refer to sequences of (reconstructed) PMP phonemes, GCs almost always refer to nondistinctive prosodic features, as well. (ii) Initially derived from PMP sequences, Gestalt Conditions may arise in any historical stage of a language, e.g. early pre-Rejang, late pre-Rejang, Proto-Rejang, or in one or all contemporary dialects (where they would be described synchronically as Morpheme Structure Conditions). GCs refer to one or all of the following: the last two syllables of the word; the accent; the pairing of the vowels (*a-i, *u-«, etc.); the shape of the final syllable (open or closed); and the velarity (= binary feature [ + velar] ) associated with the word-final consonant (if present).

It is assumed that Rejang underwent two prosodic shifts at different times in its early (before Proto-Rejang), and each prosodic shift led to 'natural' segmental changes affecting vowels and consonants; in particular, stressed vowels became strengthened, and unstressed vowels became weakened or lost (McGinn 1997). The sheer number of vocalic shifts in Rejang raises questions about the external history of the language; see section 4.4 for discussion. Other questions unfortunately cannot be answered here (but see 4.4.2). As noted by Paul Kiparsky,

What remains puzzling on all accounts is the persistence of (vocalic) shifts ... in certain languages ... and their total absence in others, such as Japanese. It has been speculated (Wallace 1975) that this persistence is ultimately traceable to certain properties of the prosodic system.

(Kiparsky 1988:383)

Early changes in the history of Rejang contributed to a high degree of lexical vowel harmony in Proto-Rejang which is largely, but not entirely, reflected in the contemporary dialects. In particular, the first nine changes listed in Table 3 are shown together with a tenth change that occurred after dialect split. All were governed by Gestalt Conditions.

Table 3: Schematic History of Proto-Rejang Vowels

Gestalt Changes in Early Pre-Rejang PMP pre-Rej Gloss__

A 1 First Stress shift (to Malay-type pattern) *manuk *ma:nuk chicken

2 Syllable reductions *daqan *dan branch

3a *a Neutralization *-V:CaC[-velar] > *-V:C«C *taNan *ta:N«n hand

3b *a Neutralization *-V:Ca # > -V:C« *mata *ma:t« eye

Gestalt Changes in Late Pre-Rejang PMP Proto-Rej Gloss

B 4 Second Stress Shift (to last syllabic V) *taNan *taN«:n hand

5 CVCV Harmony I *a-i: > i-i: *talih *tili: rope

6 CVCV Harmony II *a-u: > u-u: *sapu *supu: broom

7 CVCVC Harmony I *ä-i: > *ä-ä: *laNit *läNä:t sky

8 CVCVC Harmony II *ä-u: > *o-o: *manuk *mono:k chicken

9 CVCVC[-velar] Harmony III *i-«: > -ä-ä: *ipen *äpä:n tooth

C 10 CVCVC[+velar] Harmony IV *u-«: > o-o: = u-«: *pusej *pus«:j navel

pus«t (Rawas)

posog (Keb)

posok (PLM)

Table 3 is discussed in sections 2.4.6.3 and 3.2.1 of this paper. Table 3 schematizes the general outlines of Rejang's linguistic history presented in McGinn (1997, 1999, 2000), extended now to accommodate the new evidence from Rawas. At first, the Rawas evidence shown in (10) appears to contradict our earlier analysis, since Rawas vowels were not affected at all. Upon closer examination, however, it is clear that Rawas provides exactly the right kind of negative evidence needed to “prove the rule". McGinn (1997) claimed that the two vocalic changes illustrated in (10) were conditioned in part by the feature [+velar] associated with word-final -C (including PMP *-R, *-j, *-k ). But in Rawas, it is clear that consonantal change has intervened, namely, PMP and PR *-j became -t in Rawas, whereas PR *j > -g or -k in the other dialects. The explanation is that Rawas failed to undergo the harmonization pattern (10) parallel to the other dialects because PMP *-j had changed to -t in Rawas, in effect ‘bleeding’ the rule.

Theoretically, then, the Rejang evidence supports a theory of sound change that includes a restricted class of phonologically (and typologically) definable conditions, called here Gestalt Conditions (GC), operating over the domain of the word or word-base, perhaps in different ways at different time periods in the history of a language.

2.0 Proto-Rejang

In his famous Vergleichende Lautlehre des Austronesischen Wortschatzes (1934-1938) Otto Dempwolff presented the material as if (als ob) just three languages were necessary and sufficient to reconstruct a valid protolanguage (Volume I), and as if only eleven more languages were needed to confirm the reconstructions (Volume II). A similar simplification (on a much smaller scale to be sure) is implicit throughout this paper with respect to the reconstruction of Proto-Rejang. In fact, every feature of Proto-Rejang can be justified based on evidence from just two dialects—either Rawas and Pesisir, or Rawas and Kebanagung. As it happens, these are the only dialects that share a boundary with a dialect of Malay. By contrast, the remaining two (Musi and Lebong) may be viewed as "test dialects" with respect to our reconstructed Proto-Rejang. These two dialects occupy the political and geographic heartland in Kabupaten Rejang-Lebong. See section 4.4.3.

Of the three "criterion" dialects—Rawas, Pesisir and Kebanagung—Rawas typically provides the best (and sometimes the only direct) witness for a given feature of Proto-Rejang. In fact, the direct contributions of Pesisir and Kebanagung can be summarized in just a few sentences. Pesisir bears witness to just three features of Proto-Rejang that have been lost or obscured in Rawas. Pesisir retains: (a) -? from PR *-? and PMP *-q (=Rawas -h); (b) i and u in the end-rhymes -ia? and -ua? from PR *-i? and*-u? reflecting PMP *-iq and *-uq respectively (=Rawas -äh, -oh); and (c) -i and -u from PR *-i and *-u in the set of personal pronouns, e.g. si ' 3Psg', uku ‘1Psg’ (Rawas s«y, uk«w). Kebanagung, on the other hand, bears witness to four features of Proto-Rejang that have been lost or altered in Rawas. Thus in Kebanagung (a) h regularly reflects PR *r from PMP *r and *R in all consonant positions (=Rawas l or ? or zero in corresponding positions); (b) -i regularly reflects PR *-i from PMP *-a when the penult was *u, e.g. dui, 'two' tui, 'old' buNi 'flower' (=Rawas du«y, tu«y, buN«y); (c) -k regularly reflects PR and PMP *-k[3] (=Rawas -?); and finally (d) -g regularly reflects PR and PMP *-j (= Rawas -t).

All other features of PR are reflected with varying degrees of transparency in contemporary Rawas.

2.1 Reconstructed PR Systems

Proto-Rejang had several typologically important features: seven vowels (attested in Rawas); a high degree of vowel harmony in the lexicon--even higher than attested in any contemporary dialect; word-level stress (accent) on the final syllabic vowel (attested in all dialects); and just two diphthongs--far fewer than attested in any contemporary dialect. See section 3. for discussion.

2.1.1 Phonemic Inventory

Table 4. Proto-Rejang Consonants (23), Vowels (7) and Diphthongs (3)

Proto-Rejang Consonants Proto-Rejang Vowels

Stops & Affri- *p *t *c *k *? High *i *u

cates *b *d *z *g *j [gy][4]

Fricative *s Mid *e *« *o

Plain Nasals *m *n *N

‘Barred’ Nasals *m *n *N Low ä *a

Liquids *l *r

Semivowels *w *y Diphthongs: *iw, *uy

PR *? was glottal stop; PR *r was presumably velar liquid (reflected as h or ? or zero in contemporary dialects); PR was low, front and unrounded (reflected as ä in Rawas); and the series *m, *n, * ñ, *N represents the ‘barred nasals’ (Coady and McGinn 1983). They are regular reflexes of PMP consonant sequences *-mb-, *-nd-, *-nz- and *-Ng-, respectively.

2.1.2 Seven Vowels of Proto-Rejang

PR had an inventory of seven vowels (witnessed in Rawas): *a, *e, *i, *o, *u, *«, *ä. Given that PMP had four vowels (*i, *u, *a, *e (=schwa)) it is obvious that PR *o, *e and are innovations. Two facts are especially noteworthy about the innovating set. First, the relationship between PR and *e is problematic; in particular, most instances of PR are inherited from PMP whereas PR *e is attested only in borrowed words from Malay or from unknown sources, e.g. PR *kidek 'evil; dirty'.

Second, the following pan-dialectal constraint applies when the relevant vowels are available..

Given e or o or ä as the penultimate vowel in a word, only like vowels

are permitted in ultimate position.

The constraint governs not only native words like monok 'chicken' and Nät = leNet 'sky' but also many borrowed words like topoN 'western-style hat' (Malay topi) and mugo mugo (Malay moga-moga 'hopefully').[5] Also included are PR words of unknown origin, e.g. PR *kidek > PLM kide? = Kebanagung kidek = Rawas kede? ‘evil'.

2.1.3 PR Diphthongs and Accent

The accent (word-level stress) is assigned to the final non-syllabic vowel of the word in Proto-Rejang, as in all contemporary dialects (1.4.3). The accent almost certainly contributed to the spread of diphthongization of final vowels after dialect split. See section 3.

2.2. Proto-Rejang Diphthongs (Reflecting PMP Diphthongs)

PR diphthongs either reflect PMP diphthongs or are the result of vowel coalescence following loss of intervocalic *-q- or *-h-. Like much else in the reconstruction of Proto-Rejang, the Rawas evidence proves crucial, in part because it diverges sharply from the other dialects, and in part because it is conservative. Rawas retains PMP and PR diphthongs *iw and *uy as iw and uy, respectively. Thus, although Rawas reflects only two PMP diphthongs as diphthongs (vs. three in each of the other dialects), it does so conservatively.

This section is designed to explain the following display.

PMP Diphthongs: *iw, *uy, *aw, *ay

Regular reflexes in PR: *iw, *uy

Regular reflexes in Rawas: iw, uy

The following are the diachronic rules for each dialect..

(1) PMP *aw and *iw merged as PR *iw (witnessed by Rawas iw).

(2a) PMP *ay and *uy merged as PR *uy (witnessed by Rawas uy).

(2b) PMP sequences *-aqi and *-ahi became *ay before merging

with *uy (witnessed by Rawas uy), e.g. PMP *taqi > PR *tuy (Rawas tuy).

See Appendix: (31),(222),(243)

Consider the examples in Table 5.

Table 5: Regular Reflexes of PMP Diphthongs

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas Gloss

1. *danaw *daniw dan«w dan«w danuo dan«a daniw lake

2. *kahiw *kiiw ki«w ki«w kiuo ki«a kiiw wood

3. *qatay *atuy at«y at«y atie at«e atuy liver

4. *hapuy *upuy opoy opoy opoy opoy upuy fire

after loss of PMP intervocalic *-q-:

5. *tinaqi *t«nuy t«n«y t«n«y t«nie t«n«e t«nuy guts

See Appendix: (1),(11),(45),(61),(99),(121),(134),(177),(200),(251).

After dialect split the following changes account for the attested outcomes shown in Table 5.

Pesisir and Lebong

1) PR *iw > «w

2) PR *uy > «y

3) PR Gestalt *-u-uy > -o-oy

Musi

1) PR *uy > ie

2) PR *iw > uo

3) PR Gestalt *u-uy > o-oy (cf. (c) above)

Kebanagung

1) PR *uy > «e

2) PR *iw > «a

3) PR Gestalt *u-uy > o-oy (cf. (c) above)

Rawas (no change):

PMP/PR *iw and *uy > iw, uy respectively

2.3 Consonantal Change

The following consonantal changes can be attributed to Proto-Rejang (PR). Special reference will be made to the evidence from Rawas and Kebanagung, because (with only one exception (see (2) below) the PR reflexes can be derived on the basis of evidence from these two dialects alone (see section 2.0).

2.3.1 Summary of Consonantal Changes in Proto-Rejang (PR)

(1) PMP *h disappeared unconditionally in PR: PMP *hasaq > Rawas as«a? 'sharp'; PMP *talih > Rawas til«y 'rope'; PMP *buhek > Rawas buk 'head hair'.

Appendix: (14),(41), (45),(50),(97),(116),(118),(146),(167),(249).(200),(236).

(2) PMP *q became PR *? in word-final position and disappeared elsewhere. However, Rawas and Kebanagung display -h instead of expected glottal stop. The explanation may be attributed to the broad spectrum of contiguous languages with borrowed h for expected ? under areal pressure from Malay (Blust 1992:37). See 2.3.2.2 for discussion.

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas Gloss

*Rumaq *ruma? um«a? um«a? um«a? umah umah house

*taneq *tana? tan«a? tan«a? tan«a? tanah tanah earth

Appendix: (7),(18),(29),(32),(34),(42),(43),(44),(57),(60),(69),(85),(91),(135),

(136),(126),(150),(168),(172),(174),(187),(200),(211),(219),(243),(234),(244),(245)

(246),(253),(248),(250),(255),(256).

(3) PMP *k > PR *k (all positions) is attested by Kebanagung. In the other dialects, including Rawas, word-final PMP and PR *-k > -?, thus partially merging with PMP *-q.

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas Gloss

*anak *anak ana? ano? ana? anak ana? child

*balik *bäläk bele? bele? bele? belek bälä? return

*buhuk *buk bu? bu? bu? buk bu? head hair

Notice that in Lebong PMP/PR *a regularly became o before *-k but not *-q: *anak > ano? beside *Rumaq > um«a? 'house' (umo?** is unattested). In the phonological system of Kebanagung -k = [?]; thus buk [bu?], belek [bele?], anak [ana?] as the result of an allophonic rule (McGinn 1997:68). Phonologically, therefore, Kebanagung retains traces of earlier distinctions among PMP *-q, *-k and *-j, to which we next turn.

(4) PMP *j became PR *g between vowels but was retained as PR *-j in word-final position before splitting into Rawas -t, Kebanagung -g [k] and PLM -k [k].

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas GLOSS

A *qap«ju *p«gu p«gaw p«gaw p«g«w n.c. p«g«w gall

*najan *gän gen gen gen gen gän name

B *pusej *pusej posok posok posok posog pus«t navel

*qulej *ul«j olok olok olok olog ul«t caterpiller

*lalej *dal«j dal«k dal«k dal«k dal«g dal«t housefly

Appendix: (58),(79),(162),(170),(172),(181)

(5) PMP *p and *b remained unchanged in PR in all positions except under a specific morphological condition, namely, word-initially in transitive verbs PMP *p-[6] and *b-[7] were re-analyzed as prefixes (hence disappeared lexically).

PMP *puluq > PR *pulu? 'ten' Rawas poloh ‘ten’

PMP *piliq > PR *ili? 'choose' Rawas äläh ‘choose’

Appendix (39),(255),(85),(80),(86)

(6) PMP *w > PR *b regularly in initial position. Intervocalic PMP *w regularly disappeared in trisyllables (*ka-wanan > kan«n 'rightside). In disyllables PMP intervocalic *w was regularly retained as PR *w but (irregularly) became -b- in one known case (PR *ñabi 'soul').

PMP *wahiR > PR *biol 'water' Rawas biol

PMP *hawak > PR *awak 'body' Kebanagung awak

PMP *ñawa > PR *ñabi 'soul' Rawas ñab«y

Appendix: (12),(20),(38),(98),(102),(123),(159),(215)

(7) PMP was retained as PR word-initially but became *n between vowels.

PMP *ñamuk > PR *ñomok 'mosquito' Rawas ñomo?

PMP *ñawa > PR *ñabi 'soul' Rawas ñab«y

PMP *ma-añud > PR *monot 'float away' Kebanagung monot

Appendix: (149),(159),(160)

(8) PMP *l was retained as PR *l in all positions, as attested in Rawas. In all dialects except Rawas word-final PMP and PR *l disappeared.

PMP *gatel > PR *gatal Rawas gatal 'itch'

Appendix: (19),(26),(27),(28),(35),(46),(47),(48),(59),(78),(102),(104),(128),

(129),(131),(135),(136),(173),(240),(245)[8]

(9) PMP *r (presumably alveolar) is reflected as PR *r (presumed to be velar or possibly uvular; hereafter [+velar] for concreteness) in all consonant positions; after dialect split PR *r developed regularly in each dialect. In Kebanagung PR *r > h in all three positions; in the other dialects PR *r > ? between vowels and zero elsewhere.

PMP *rimba > PR *rima Kebanagung himo 'jungle'

PMP *zari > PR *ziri Kebanagung jih«y 'finger'

PMP *tirus > PR tirus Kebanagung tihus 'tapering'[9]

PMP *bener > PR *b«n«r Kebanagung b«n«h 'true'

Appendix: (6),(30),(190),(138),(207), (231),(258); cf. also (194), (230)

(10) PMP *R was retained as PR *r in initial position. Intervocalically and word-finally, PMP *R split into PR *r and *l under complex conditions as discussed in section 2.3.2.3.

PMP *Ratus > PR *rotos Kebanagung hotos 'hundred'

PMP *baqeRu > PR *b«lu Kebanagung b«l«w 'new'

PMP *libeR > PR *lib«r Kebanagung lib«h 'wide'

Appendix: (20),(29),(32),(36),(40),(51),(67), (70),(71),(109),(130),(131),

(134),(175),(188),(193),(200),(202),(221),(228),(234),(250)

(11) PMP *z became PR *d word-initially in CV(C)VC canons; elsewhere PMP *z was retained as PR *z except in the intervocalic cluster *-nz- which collapsed as PR * ñ. Schematically:

PMP *z > d / #__V(C)VC[10]

*z > z elsewhere

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas Gloss

A *zalan *dal«n dal«n dal«n dal«n dal«n dal«n road

*zaRum *dolom dolom dolom dolom dolom dolom needle

*zaqit *m«n-dät m«net m«net m«net m«net m«nät sew

B *zari *ziri ji?ay ji?ay ji?«y jih«y ji?«y finger

C *quzan *uz«n uj«n uj«n uj«n uj«n uj«n rain

*tazem *taz«m taj«m taj«m taj«m taj«m taj«m sharp

*pinzem *iñ«m «m «m «m «m «m borrow

Aslo Appendix: (114),(244)

(12) Voiced stops devoiced in final position.

PMP *bukid > PR bukit 'hill' Rawas bukit

PMP *tunked > PR *tokot 'staff, cane' Rawas tokot

(13) A number of processes resulted in original CVCCV(C) and CVCeCVC canons reducing to CVCVC or CVCV template.

(a) "Prenasalized voiceless obstruents reduced to the simple obstruent." (Blust 1984:428)

PMP kempu > PR *k«pu 'grandchild' Rawas k«p«w

Appendix: (107),(201),(204)

(b) "Prenasalized voiced obstruents shifted to the corresponding barred nasal."

PMP *embun > PR *«mun 'cloud' Rawas «mun

Appendix: (75),(76), (86), (166),(180),(190),(233),(235); see also (87)

(c) "The first of successive consonants in a reduplicated monosyllable was dropped." (Blust 1984:428)

PMP *tektek > PR *t«tok ‘chop, hack’ Rawas t«to?

PMP *tuktuk > PR *tutuk 'pound rice' Rawas tutu?

(d) CVCeCVC trisyllables regularly reduced to CVCCVC (schwa syncope[11]) followed by cluster reduction. (See also 2.4.1)

PMP *tupelak > *tuplak > PR *tulak 'push; reject' Rawas tula?

PMP *timeRaq > *timRaq > PR *tima? 'tin' Rawas timah

(14) The following irregular consonant changes have been noted in all dialects, hence presumably occurred in PR.

1) PMP *k- > PR *g: PMP *kutu > PR *gutu (Rawas gut«w) 'head louse'

2) PMP *d- > PR *t: PMP *deNeR > PR *t«Noa (Keban t«Noa) 'hear'

3) PMP *-j- > PR zero: PMP *pajay > PR *pay (Rawas pay 'rice plant'

4) PMP *l- > PR *d: *lalej > PR *dal«j (Rawas dal«t) 'housefly'

5) PMP *-l- > PR *n: PMP *qateluR > PR *t«nol (Rawas t«nol) 'egg'

6) PMP *-w > zero: PMP *laRiw > PR *lili (Rawas lil«y `run'

7) PMP *n- > PR *l-: PMP *nipis > PR *m«-lipis (Rawas m«lipis) ‘thin’

8) PMP *b- > PR *-w-: PMP *bahi ‘female’ > Rawas root -wuy in s«lawuy

‘woman’.

9) PMP *-p- > PR -b-: PMP *ma-kapal > PR *k«bol > Rawas k«bol 'thick'

10) PMP *-nd- > -d-: PMP *pandak > PR *p«dak > Rawas p«da? 'short'

In addition, the following irregular consonant changes were introduced after dialect split in the named dialects.

11) PR *b- > zero in PLM: PMP/PR *busuk > PLM usu? 'rotten'

12) PR *r- > zero in Kebanagung: PR *ruma? > Kebanagung umah 'house'

All other consonants remain unaltered in all dialects.

2.3.2 Residual Problems With Reference To Consonantal Changes in PR

This section discusses problems arising from the proposed PR consonant system. The solutions offered are somewhat tentative.

2.3.2.1 Split of PMP *R into PR *r and *l

All contemporary dialects contain one liquid--lateral l -- from two sources in PMP, namely, *l and *R. Some of the derivations are complex, and there are residues from the proposed analysis which are duly noted.

As noted in the previous section:

(1) PMP *l was retained as PR *l in all positions (attested by Rawas l ).

(2) PMP *r was retained as PR *r in all positions (attested by Kebanagung h).

These retention facts, unproblematic in themselves, are crucial for the analysis of the fate of PMP *R.

(3) Word-initially PMP *R- was retained as PR *r- ([+velar]). PR *r- is reflected by Kebanagung h- corresponding to zero in the other dialects. An unexplained loss of PR *r- occurs in Kebanagung umah (expected humah**) from PMP *Rumah 'house'.

PMP PR P&L Musi Keban Rawas GLOSS

*Rakit *rakit eket eket heket äkät raft

*Ratus *rotos otos otos hotos otos hundred

*Rumaq *(r)uma? um«a? um«a? umah umah house

(4) Between vowels PMP *-R- split between PR *-r- and *-l- conditioned by a morph-shape (Gestalt) condition. According to McGinn (1997:87):

PMP intervocalic *R became l except in the following two environments:

(a) *-R- disappeared in trisyllables

(b) *-R- > *-h- in the environment C1V _2VC3 when the initial consonant was

a noncoronal obstruent (*p-, *b-, *k-, (?)*g-)

In this paper we have substituted PR *-r- for the *-h- of the earlier analysis, but the substance is unchanged. Here is the data presented in McGinn (1997:87, Table 10) updated to include the Rawas data and new reconstructions (with *-r- replacing *-h-).

Table 6: Split of PMP *-R-

PMP PR Pes & Leb Musi Keban. Rawas Gloss

A *keRiN *k«riN k«?iN k«?iN k«hiN ki?iN dry

*peRes *p«r«s n.c. p«?«s n.c. n.c. squeeze

*buRuk *buruk bu?u? bu?u? buhuk n.c. decayed

B *waRej *wal«t[12] bal«t bal«t bal«t n.c. root, vine

*waRi *wili bilay bil«y bil«y bil«y day

*baqeRu *b«lu b«law b«l«w b«l«w b«l«w new

*daRaq *dalaq dal«a? dal«a? dalah dalah blood

*zaRum *dalum dolom dolom dolom dolom needle

*laRiw *lili lilay lil«y n.d. lil«y run

*beRay *luy l«y lie l«e luy give

*ma-iRaq *milaq mil«a? mil«a? n.c. n.c. red

*qasiRa *sili silay sil«y sil«y n.c. salt

C *beReqat *b«r«t b«?«t b«?«t b«h«t n.c. heavy

-- *b«n«g b«n«k (Leb) n.c. n.c. b«n«k heavy

*deRes *d«r«s n.c. d«?«s d«h«s d«?«s swift

(current)

The Rawas reflexes of PMP *-R- show the identical split as Pesisir, Lebong and Musi. Only Kebanagung is different, exactly as reported in McGinn (1997), which now gains support from the new evidence. Set C contains two unexplained residues of the analysis (expected b«l«t** and d«l«s**). Probably this pair of outcomes can be explained as regularized early borrowings from Malay deras and berat.

(5) In word-final position PMP *-R split between PR *-r and *-l.

Table 7: Split of PMP *-R

PMP PR PL Musi Keban Rawas GLOSS

A *wahiR *biol bioa bioa bioa biol water

*niuR *niol nioa nioa nioa niol coconut

*ikuR *ikol ikoa ikoa ikoa iko? (borr) tail

*dapuR *dopol dopoa dopoa dopoa dopol hearth

*qateluR *t«nol t«noa t«noa t«noa t«nol egg

*deNeR *t«Nol t«Noa t«Noa t«Noa n.c. hear

B *huluR *ulur ulua oloa uluh ulua lower

*qapuR *upur upua opoa opoh upua chalk

*tiDuR *tidur tidua tidoa tiduh tidua sleep

*libeR *lib«r lib«a lib«a lib«h lib«a wide

*qiliR *ilir n.c. elea ilih n.c. downstream

C *bibiR *bibir bibia bebea (Nus) bibia lips

In McGinn (1997:86)--which lacked Rawas data--the question was: Under what conditions did PMP *-R become h in Kebanagung and zero in the other dialects? Given our analysis so far, the right question seems to be: Under what conditions did PMP *-R split into PR *-l and *-r ? Consider the complementation facts in sets A and B. All the forms in set A display PR *-ol and all the forms in set B show PR *-ir, *-ur or *-«r. PMP *-R thus evidently changed to PR *-l before derived *o, but became PR *-r elsewhere. This analysis assumes that pre-Rejang *-oR appeared before the consonantal split. Thus the split of PMP *R into PR *r, *l and zero was overwhelmingly regular.

2.3.2.2 Malay Influence in Early pre-Rejang Reflected in PR Consonants

(1) Three PR kin terms show irregular final *-k instead of expected *-? from PMP *-q. E.g. Kebanagung bapak ‘father’, kakak ‘elder sibling’, mamak ‘mother’s brother’ occur instead of expected bap«a?**, mam«a?**, kak«a?**. The explanation for the irregularity is early borrowing from Malay bapak, mamak, kakak. See McGinn (1997) for discussion.

(2) Rawas and Kebanagung show -h regularly corresponding to -? from PMP *-q in the other dialects. Whereas this derives in a straightforward way from PMP *q, it is less clear how it relates to Proto-Rejang. There are three possible ways to account for the correspondence set ?=?=?=h=h, none particularly satisfactory. The set reflects either (i) PR *-h from *-q, (ii) PR *-? from *-q, or (iii) a retention, namely PR *-q from *-q. Probably (i) should be rejected on the grounds that *h > ? is phonetically unlikely in the Austronesian world, if not universally. And possibility (iii) offers no new insight and therefore should be available only as a last resort. That leaves (ii), which like (i) suffers the consequence of an unlikely regular change, namely *? > -h in two Rejang dialects (Rawas and Kebanagung). However, there is another fact to be considered based on a suggestion by Blust (1992:37). Blust has suggested that -h has replaced the regular reflexes of PMP *-q in many languages over a broad and not always contiguous area in Java, Sumatra, and Borneo. Since this pattern represents a partial irregularity in language after language, it may be explained as an areal feature spread by the prestige of Malay, whose early history included the change *-q > h (see Adelaar 1992). Accepting this theory, and applying it to Rejang, I assume that PMP *-q became *-? in Proto-Rejang, which then was replaced by -h in Kebanagung and Rawas via borrowing from Malay. (Note that these two Rejang dialects just happen be the only ones in direct contact with Malay.) One advantage of the borrowing theory is that it helps to account for the peculiar distribution of Rawas h, which occurs only word-finally. This fact would be surprising except for the assumption that Rawas -h is a borrowed phoneme.

In contrast, Kebanagung shows h in all positions, reflecting partial merger of PMP *R and *r as PR *r. Continuing with the consequences of the borrowing theory, in Kebanagung (but not Rawas) our presumed borrowed -h extended in its distribution through replacement of PR *r by h in all positions. (In the other dialects, PR *r became ? between vowels and zero elsewhere.) As a consequence of all these developments, r does not occur in native words, but is a sure sign of borrowing in Rejang, e.g. s«rgap 'attack' (all dialects) from Malay sergap.

3) Two outcomes showing PR intervocalic *-l- might be explained in terms of early Malay influence.

Malay PR Rawas PMP Gloss

lari *lili lil«y *laRiw run

kura-kura *kuli kul«y -- turtle

Early borrowing from Malay lari as PR *lili is the probable source of Kebanagung and Rawas lil«y, which would account for the apparent loss of *-w (cf. PMP *laRiw ‘run’). Furthermore, Rawas -l- corresponding to Malay -r- suggests that the pre-Rejang form was *laRi (with velar or uvular *R) which occurs in Pagar Alam Malay.

4) Rawas añut (expected monot**) from Malay hanyut 'drift away' from PMP *ma-qañud.

5) PM b«?«t 'heavy', Keb b«h«t 'heavy' (expected b«l«t**) from Malay berat 'heavy'; cf. PMP *beReqat. The origin of PR *b«n«g (> Rawas, Lebong b«n«k 'heavy') is unknown.

6) PMP *ajeN > PR *araN (Rawas a?aN) 'charcoal' (expected ag«N**) from Malay arang.

***

7) PMP *tawad > PR *taw«r (Keban taw«h) 'haggle' (expected taw«t**) from Malay tawar.

2.4. Changes Affecting PMP Vowels in Proto-Rejang

The PMP inventory of four vowels (*i, *u, *a, *e (=schwa)) evolved into a seven-vowel system in PR by the addition of PR *o and (= low front unrounded) plus a new phoneme PR *e introduced by early borrowing (see 5.0)). It is interesting that no new diphthongs developed between PMP and PR. Nineteen vocalic changes separating PMP and Proto-Rejang (PR) are illustrated in this section (2.4) followed by discussion (2.5). Rawas and Kebanagung dialect evidence is especially prominent in this section for reasons given in 2.0. (See section 3. for seventeen additional vocalic changes separating PR from contemporary Rawas.)

2.4.1 PR Reflexes of PMP *a (=7)

PMP *-a is reflected as zero and six PR vowels (all except *e), namely: *a,* ä, *i, *u, *o, and *«. The changes were conditioned by the accent in every case.

a) Vocalic Changes Reconstructed For Early Pre-Rejang And Conditioned By Malay-type Accent[13]

(1) *a > Ø *qaCV:CV(C) (prepenultimate *V unstressed)

*ma-V:CV(C) (prepenultimate *V unstressed)

The prepenultimate sequence PMP *qa- disappeared regularly. Moreover, in PMP affixes *mi-, and *um- (for which see below) and *ma- the vowel disappeared before a base-initial vowel.

PMP *qapeju > PR *p«gu: > Rawas p«g«w

PMP *ma-iRaq > PR *mila? > Pesisir mil«a? 'red'

Appendix: (136),(146),(149),(150),(172),(200),(202),(221)

(2a) *a > *« *CaCV:CV(C) (prepenultimate *V unstressed)

PMP *baqeRu > PR *b«lu > Rawas b«l«w 'new'

Appendix: (17),(28),(29),(40),(130),(136),(145),(149),(173),(200),(226)

More generally, prepenultimate PMP *a, *i and *u became PR *« except where schwa syncope (2.3.1 (14)) applied first.

PMP *tuqelaN > PR *t«lan > Rawas t«lan 'bone'

PMP *tinaqi > PR *t«nuy > Rawas t«nuy 'intestines'

To anticipate somewhat, two other cases of schwa syncope 'bled' prepenultimate neutralization of PMP *i and *u.[14]

PMP *tupelak > *tuplak > PR *tulak > Rawas tula? 'push

PMP *timeRaq > *timRaq > PR *tima? > Rawas timah 'tin'

(2b) *a > *« *-V:CaC[-velar] (ultimate *a unstressed)

PMP *bulan > bu:l«n > PR *bul«:n > Rawas bul«n 'moon'

PMP *anak >*a:nak > PR *ana:k > Rawas ana? 'child'

PMP *takebas > *t«ba:s > PR *t«ba:s > Rawas t«bas 'clear-cut'

PMP *daqan >*dan > PR *dan > Rawas dan 'branch'

Appendix: (3),(43),(46),(47),(57),(59),(60),(69),(81),(88),(97),(98),(104),(110),

(115),(123),(117),(136),(139),(145),(150),(164),(166),(167),(168),(180),(193),(204),

(213),(215),(219),(225),(226),(229),(234),(238),(239),(246),(253),(256); See section 2.4.6.1 for discussion.

(2c) *a > *« *-VCa (all vowels unstressed in function words; see 3.3)

PMP *(k)ita > PR *it« > Rawas it« 1Ppl(incl)

PMP *ni-a > PR *n« > Rawas n« 3Psg(agent-possessive)

PMP *duha > pre-Rejang *du:«

Appendix: (113),(156)

See (3a) below for further changes affecting content words, and section 3.3 for pronouns and other function words.

b) Vocalic Changes Reconstructed For Late Pre-Rejang And Conditioned By Contemporary Accent

(3a) *-a > *-« > *-«: > *i (last step applied to output of (2c))

*-V:Ca > *-V:C« > *-VC«: > -VCi:

1 2 3 4

PMP *duha > *du:« > du«: > PR *dui: > Kebanagung dui 'two'

PMP *tuqa > *tu:« > tu«: > PR *tui: > Kebanagung tui ‘old’

PMP *buNa > *bu:N« > buN«: > PR *buNi: > Kebanagung buNi ‘flower’

Appendix: (49),(72),(140),(159),(238),(130)

Outcomes (2b), (2c) and (3a) are obviously closely related. When described together, they bring several issues into focus, including the following. (i) 1st Stress Shift to Malay-type pattern (ii) 2nd Stress Shift to contemporary pattern; (iii) neutralization of unstressed vowels; (iv) strengthening (eventual diphthongization) of stressed vowels; and (v) non-participation of grammatical function words (=clitics) in vocalic strengthening rules. Table 8 displays the skeletal history just described. Crucially for the theory developed here and in McGinn (1997), word-level stress (accent) is assigned metrically within the disyllabic base; thus monosyllables like ba were exempt (see 4.3). Stressed vowels are represented by a colon.

Table 8a: Two Stress Shifts In Relation to Three Regular

PR Outcomes for PMP *-a in Open Final Syllables

PMP (accent unknown) *teka *ba *(k)ita *duha

Early pre-Rej (Malay-type accent) *t«ka: *ba *it« *du:«

Late pre-Rej (contemporary accent) *t«ka: *ba *it« *du«:

Proto-Rejang *t«ka: *ba *it« *dui:

Contemporary Kebanagung t«ko ba it« dui

Contemporary Rawas t«kaw ba it« du«y

Gloss 'come' particle '1Ppl(incl)' 'two'

The crucial step is underlined in Table 8a. Under the influence of the reconstructed Malay-type accent in early pre-Rejang, word-final vowels were unstressed when the penultimate vowel was *« (schwa); otherwise the penultimate vowel was stressed. These assumptions underlie the following derivation of the Rawas diphthong «y from PR *i and PMP *-a.

PR Rawas

PMP *-a > -« > -«: > -i: > «y

| | | | |

(accent unstressed stressed stressed stressed

unknown)

First, unstressed *-a regularly became *« (schwa). Then, after the accent shifted (=late pre-Rejang), stressed *-«: became *-i and later (after dialect split) became «y in Rawas (3.2). The reason the pronoun it«~t« escaped diphthongization is that all function words (including pronouns) behaved like unstressed or de-stressed clitics in Proto-Rejang, hence systematically escaped the earliest vocalic changes affecting stressed vowels. See McGinn (1997) and section 2.4.6.1 below.

These same themes come into play in the PR outcomes shown in (2b), as shown in Table 8b below. Again the major issue concerns the conditions for neutralization of unstressed reflexes of PMP final-syllable *a--in this case, when the final syllable was closed (final -C = non-velar); it did not affect stressed vowels (*t«ba:s) nor monosyllables, which bear no metrical stress at all.

Table 8b: Two Stress Shifts In Relation to Two Regular

PR Outcomes for PMP *-a in Closed Final Syllables

PMP (accent unknown) *hasap *anak *daqan *takebas

Early pre-Rej (Malay-type accent) *a:s«p *a:nak *dan *t«ba:s

Late pre-Rej (contemporary accent) *as«:p *ana:k *dan *t«ba:s

Proto-Rejang *as«p *ana:k *dan *t«ba:s

Contemporary Kebanagung as«:p ana:k dan t«ba:s

Contemporary Rawas as«:p ana:? dan t«ba:s

Gloss 'smoke' 'child' 'branch 'clear-cut'

The crucial step is underlined in Table 8b. The unity of the processes displayed in Tables 8a and 8b was expressed as a single formula in McGinn (1997). This unity remains valid after reviewing the Rawas data. See 2.4.6.1 and 4.2.2.

(3b) *a > *i *-aCi: (*a unstressed)

PMP *talih > *ta:li > PR *tili: > Rawas til«y 'rope'

Appendix: (38),(40),(258)

(4) *a > *u *-aCu: (*a unstressed)

PMP *sapu > *sa:pu > PR *supu: > Rawas sup«w 'broom'

Appendix: (52),(119),(247)

(5) *a > *o *-aCu:C (*a unstressed)

PMP *manuk > PR *mono:k > Rawas mono? 'chicken'

Appendix: (70),(71),(148),(149),(191),(205)

(6) *a > *ä *-aCi:C (*a unstressed)

PMP *laNit > * la:Nit > PR *läNä:t > Rawas Nät 'sky'

Appendix: (25),(26),(188),(124)(144)

(7) *a > *a Elsewhere, PMP *a was retained as PR *a.

Two noteworthy retentions of PMP *a concern (i) monosyllables such as PMP *ba, and (ii) 'oxytone' bases with open final syllables, such as PMP *t«ka. Such forms regularly retained *-a in PR (Table 8a above); after dialect split PR *-a was retained in monosyllables (all dialects), but *-a became aw in Rawas disyllabic bases (corresponding to -o in the other dialects--perhaps with *aw as intermediate step).

PMP *teka > PR *t«ka: > Kebanagung t«ko = Rawas t«kaw 'come'

PMP *ba > PR *ba > Kebanagung ba = Rawas ba 'emphatic particle'.

Appendix: (106),(128),(190),(218),(224),(233); cf. (56)

2.4.2 PR Reflexes of PMP *e (schwa)[15] (=5)

PMP *e (schwa) is reflected as zero and four PR vowels, namely: *a, *o, *ä, *«. The changes occurred in late pre-Rejang and were conditioned by the contemporary accent. In all contemporary Rejang dialects (hence PR) the accent falls predictably on the final syllabic vowel of the word.

(1) *e (schwa) > Ø *«CV:C (schwa unstressed)

PMP *emis > *«mi:s > PR *mis > Rawas mis 'sweet'

PMP *mi-hepi > *mi-«pi: > PR *mipi > Rawas mip«y 'dream'

Appendix: (146),(147),(167)

(2) *e (schwa) > *a *-VC«:C (schwa stressed)

PMP *taneq > PR *tana:? > Rawas tanah 'earth'

Appendix: (135),(211); cf. (78),(252)

(3) *e (schwa) > *o *-«C«:C[+velar] (schwa stressed)

PMP *tektek > PR *t«to:k > Rawas t«to? 'chop, hack'

PMP *wahiR > *w«y«R > *w«yoR > PR biol > Rawas biol ‘water’

Appendix: (80),(223),(227); see 2.4.6.4 for discussion.

(4) *e (schwa) > *ä *-iC«:C[-velar] (schwa stressed)

PMP *ipen > PR *äpä:n > Rawas äpän 'tooth

Appendix: (14),(15),(16),(86),(131),(143); see 3.2.1 for discussion.

(5) *e (schwa) > *« (elsewhere)

PMP *bales > PR *bal«:s > Rawas bal«s 'reply'

PMP *lesuN > PR *l«su:N > Rawas l«suN 'mortar'

2.4.3 PR Reflexes of PMP *i (=4)

PMP *i is reflected as zero and three PR vowels, namely: *ä, *«, *i. The changes were conditioned by the accent in every case.

a) Vocalic Changes Reconstructed For Early Pre-Rejang And Conditioned By Malay-type Accent

(1) *i > *« *CiCV:CV(C) (prepenultimate *V unstressed)

PMP *tinaqi > *t«naqi > *t«nai > PR *t«nuy > Rawas t«nuy 'intestines'

cf. PMP *timeRaq > *timRaq > PR *tima? > Pesisir tim«a? 'tin'

Appendix: (3); see 2.4.1 for discussion.

b) Vocalic Changes Reconstructed For Late Pre-Rejang And Conditioned By the Contemporary Accent

(1b) *i > « *CiCV:(C) (*i unstressed)

PMP *lima > PR *l«ma: > Rawas l«maw 'five'

PMP *silun > PR *s«lo:n > Kebanagung s«lon 'claw'

PMP *gilap > PR g«l«:p > Pesisir g«l«p 'flash'

Appendix: (81),(128),(199); see Blust (1984:437) and below n. 18.

(2) *i > *ä *-aCi:C (*i stressed)

PMP *laNit > PR *läNä:t > Rawas Nät'sky'

Appendix: (25),(26),(124),(144),(188); see 2.4.6.3 for discussion.

(3) *i > Ø niV (all vowels unstressed in function words)

PMP *ni-a > *na > PR *n« > Rawas n« '3Psg (agt-poss)'

PMP *ni-hu > *niu > PR *nu > Kebanagung nu '2Sg (agt-poss)'

See 3.3 for discussion.

2.4.4 PR Reflexes of PMP *u (=4)

PMP *u is reflected as zero and three PR vowels, namely: *o, *«, *u. The changes were conditioned by the accent in every case.

a) Vocalic Changes Reconstructed For Early Pre-Rejang And Conditioned By Malay-type Accent

(1) *u > Ø *uCV:CV(C) (prepenultimate *V unstressed)

PMP *um-imem > PR *min«m > Kebanagung menem ‘drink’

Appendix: (143),(150); see 2.4.1(1).

(2) *u > *« *CuCV:CV(C) (prepenultimate *V unstressed)

PMP *tuqelaN > PR *t«la:n > Rawas t«lan 'bone'

Appendix: (219); see 2.4.1 for discussion.

b) Vocalic Changes Reconstructed For Late Pre-Rejang And Conditioned By the Contemporary Accent

(3a) *u > *o *-aCu:C (*u stressed)

PMP *manuk > PR *mono:k > Rawas mono? 'chicken'

Appendix: (70),(71),(148),(149),(191),(205)

(3b) *u > *o *-i(C)uC[+velar]

PMP *ikuR > PR *iko:l > Kebanagung ikoa 'tail'

PMP *niuR > *nio:R > PR *niol > Rawas niol 'coconut'

Appendix: (84),(157); cf. also (87)

(4) *u > *u (elsewhere)

PMP *kamu > PR *kumu > Rawas kum«w 'you Sg (honorific)' (3.2.3)

PMP *sapu > PR *supu: > Rawas sup«w 'broom' (3.2.3)

PMP *lesuN > PR *l«su:N > Rawas l«suN 'mortar'

PMP *buhek > PR *buk > Rawas bu? 'head hair'

PMP *quzan > PR *uz«n > Rawas uj«n 'rain'

2.4.5 Low-Frequency Vocalic Changes and Other Residues

Although low-frequency changes may be inappropriate to describe in terms of a 'regularity hypothesis', they may be viewed as typologically characteristic of the language in question. This section lists a number of analytic residues, including cases wherein early changes triggered re-syllabification in Proto-Rejang.

1) The mid front vowel PR *e is not derivable from PMP. It is assumed to have existed in PR as an early borrowing from unknown sources. More generally, accounting historically for the contemporary contrast between e and ä in Rawas is a major problem discussed in 5.0.

2) A number of cases of vowel coalescence followed the loss of PMP intervocalic laryngeals.

a) Intermediate *CaiC > PR *CäC or *C«y«C

PMP *paqit > *pait > PR *pät > Rawas pät (=PLMK pet) 'bitter'.

PMP *nahik > *naik > PR *näk > Rawas ? = PLM ne? = K nek) 'climb'.

However, in at least one other example *CaiC > C«y«C (Blust 1984:429).

PMP *wahiR > *waiR > *w«y«R > *w«yoR > PR *biol > Rawas biol ‘water’

cf. PMP *lain > PR *lain > Pesisir lain ‘other’.

Note that parallel changes did not affect *CauC.

PMP *taqun > PR *taun > PLMK taun 'year' (Rawas ton)

PMP *lahud > PR *laut > Rawas laut 'sea'.

PMP *daun > PR *daun > Rawas daun

b) Intermediate *Cau > PR *Co

PMP *kahu > *kau > PR *ko > PLMK ko '2Psg' pronoun. (Later in Rawas PR *ko underwent lexical replacement by kab«n. See 3.3.2.)

c) Intermediate *Cai > PR *Cuy

PMP *bahi > *bai > PR *s«la-wuy > Rawas s«lawuy 'woman'

PMP *tinaqi > *tinai > PR *t«nuy 'stomach'. See 2.1.3.

cf. PMP *ay regularly became *uy (2.2)

3) Two words ending with diphthongs underwent different vocalic changes than words ending with simple consonants.

a) PMP *kahiw > *kaiw > PR *kiiw > Rawas kiiw ‘wood’ (not käw**) contrasts with the regular outcome for intermediate CaiC.

b) PMP *hapuy > *apuy > PR *upuy > Rawas upuy ‘fire’ (not PR opoy**) contrasts with the regular outcome for intermediate *CaCuC > CoCoC, e.g. *manuk > PR *monok 'chicken'. See discussion of vowel harmonization in 2.4.6.

5) Another Gestalt Condition triggered *u-Lowering (McGinn (1997:82).

*u > o /iC _ C [+velar]# :where [+velar] = reflexes

| of PMP velars and *R

[-stress]

This rule accounts for PR *o from PMP *u in the outcomes shown below.

PMP pre-Rej PR Musi P&L Keban. Rawas Gloss

-- *i:ndok *inok ino? ino? inok ino? mother

*biluk *i:lok *ilok ilo? ilo? n.c. belo?[16] turn, veer

*niuR *ni:oR *niol nioa nioa nioa niol coconut

Elsewhere in the same environment PMP *u is reflected as PR u, e.g. PMP *buRuk > PLMK bu?u? ‘decayed’; PMP *lesuN > PR *l«suN (Rawas l«suN ‘mortar’.

The rule presumably occurred in early pre-Rejang, hence the GC included the Malay-type accent. Thus, unstressed ultimate *u was lowered to o when preceded by high front *i and followed by a velar or *R and word boundary. The change involved a degree of `action at a distance' since *u was affected only when the (stressed) penult was *i. See McGinn (1997, Table 7) for discussion.

5) The PMP end-rhyme *-el became PR *-al in the only known example.

PMP PR PLM Keb Rawas Gloss

*gatel *gatal gata gata gatal itch

6) The PMP end-rhyme *-ej was retained as PR *-«j in the only known example. In contrast, a parallel end-rhyme, namely PMP *-eq, regularly became PR *-a?.

PMP PR PLM Keb Rawas Gloss

*lalej *dal«j dal«k dal«g dal«t housefly

cf. *taneq *tana? tan«a? tanah tanah earth

7) PMP *-a is irregularly reflected as PR *-a (expected *-i) in one form: PMP *dada > PR *dada ‘chest; breast.

2.4.6 Discussion: Toward ‘Perfect' Harmony in the Proto-Rejang Lexicon

All Rejang dialects exhibit a high degree of vowel harmony in the lexicon. Yet the degree of vowel harmony may have been greater at an earlier stage between PMP and PR, called pre-Rejang in McGinn (1999). In this paper, the middle ground is occupied by Proto-Rejang, where two systems of vowel harmony split the lexicon, as follows.

System A System B

[non-*a] [based on *a]

*i *u

*e (*«) *o (*«)

*a

Note that any change producing schwa within the disyllabic word-base automatically harmonized it, because schwa was--and is--the neutral vowel (thus equally at home in System A or System B); and also because all prepenultimate vowels (e.g. affixal vowels) were schwa in PR, as in contemporary dialects. Note too that whereas harmonization of PMP *a-i and *a-u vowel pairs as PR *u-u or *o-o and *i-i or ä-ä, respectively, affected previously un-harmonic vowel pairs, other PMP vowel pairs were in effect 'pre-harmonized' (e.g. PMP and PR *a-a, *u-i, *u-«, *i-«, *«-u among others); moreover, other PMP vowel pairs underwent vocalic shifting without thereby failing to contribute to the emerging harmony of the lexicon, including two vocalic dissimilation changes. Thus the outcomes of PMP *«-«C[+velar] > PR *«-o (e.g. PMP *t«kt«k > PR *t«tok 'chop, hack') and PMP *i-uC[+velar] > *i-oC (e.g. PMP *niuR > PR *niol 'coconut') satisfied System A.

Readers can satisfy themselves with respect to the degree of vowel harmony of the reconstructed PR lexicon by simply perusing the Appendix, where it will be noticed that a substantial set of unharmonic PR forms reflect PMP *-a as *-i (e.g. *mati from PMP *mata 'eye'). But the members of this set derive from etyma that were harmonized earlier, e.g. pre-Rejang *mat«. We turn to this topic next.

2.4.6.1 From PMP to Early Pre-Rejang: Some Early Vocalic Changes

Probably the earliest set of changes leading to vowel harmony effectuated neutralization of last-syllable PMP *a in open and closed syllables. In particular, after the shift to the Malay-type stress pattern, last-syllable PMP *-a and *-aC became *-« and *«C, respectively. Although the conditions are complex in both cases, they can be captured by a single formula (McGinn 1997). First, consider each change individually.

i) *-V:CaC[-velar] > *-V:C«C[-velar]

PMP *quzan > pre-Rejang *u:z«n 'rain' (Table 8b above and Table 9A below)

ii) *-V:Ca > *-V:C«

PMP *mata > pre-Rejang *ma:t« ' eye' (Table 8a above and 2.4.1)

When the final syllable was closed, the change failed to occur under the following conditions: when *a was stressed (as in intermediate pre-Rejang *t«ba:s 'clear-cut'); when *a stood alone in a monosyllable (where the concept of word-level stress differentiation is theoretically vacuous, as in pre-Rejang *dan 'branch'); and when the final consonant was velar (pre-Rejang *a:nak). See Table 9B. When the final syllable was open, on the other hand, as in PMP *mata, the change failed to occur under the following conditions: when *a was stressed (as in pre-Rejang *t«ka:); and when *a was preceded by a consonant cluster (as in pre-Rejang *ti:mba). See Table 9A.

These effects can be represented in the following formula (repeated from McGinn (1997:75)).

*a > « /-V(C) _ _ (C[-velar])# (Malay-type accent)

|

[-stress]

Table 9A: Partial Merger of PMP *a and *e as PR *«

When the final Syllable Was Open

PMP pre-Rej PR Pes Leb Musi Keb Rawas Gloss

*duha *du:« *dui: duay duay du«y dui du«y two

*mata *ma:t« *mati: matay matay mat«y mat«y mat«y eye

*teka *t«ka: *t«ka: t«ko t«ko t«ko t«ko t«kaw come

*timba *ti:mba *tima: timo timo timo timo timaw pail

Appendix: (17),(28),(45),(49),(72),(106),(128),(130),(156),(159),(190),(197),

(202), (216) (218),(224),(233),(238)

Table 9A: Partial Merger of PMP *a and *e as PR *«

When the final Syllable Was Closed

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban. Rawas Gloss

A *hasap *as«p as«p as«p as«p as«p as«p smoke

*panas *pan«s pan«s pan«s pan«s pan«s pan«s hot

*taNan *taN«n taN«n taN«n taN«n taN«n taN«n hand

*quzan *uz«n uj«n uj«n uj«n uj«n uj«n rain

B *anak *anak ana/ ano? ana/ anak anak child

*hawak *awak awa/ awo? awa/ awak awak body

*panzaN *pañaN pañaN pañaN pañaN pañaN pañaN long

*hisaN *isaN isaN isaN isaN isaN isaN gills

*t«baN *t«baN n.d. t«baN t«baN n.d. t«baN fell (tree)

C. *daRaq *dala? dal«a? dal«a/ dal«a/ dalah dalah blood

*dilaq *dila? dil«a? dil«a/ dil«a/ dilah dilah tongue

*ma-iRaq *mila? mil«a? mil«a/ mil«a/ n.c. n.c. red

*Rumaq *(r)uma? um«a? um«a/ um«a/ umah umah house

D *hekan *kan kan kan kan kan kan fish

*daqan *dan dan dan dan dan dan branch

*hepat *pat pat pat pat pat pat four

*qayam *yam yam yam yam yam yam toy

E. *tebaN *t«baN n.d. t«baN t«baN n.d. t«baN fell (tree)

*takebas *t«bas t«bas t«bas t«bas t«bas t«bas clear-cut

*tuqelaN *t«lan t«lan t«lan t«lan t«lan t«lan bone

Appendix: (3),(18),(22),(27),(32),(43),(46),(47),(57),(59),(60), (79),(88),(96),

(104),(105),(117),(123),(139),(150),(151),(164),(166),(168),(180),(193),(195),

(204),(207),(213),(215),(219),(225),(226),(229),(234), (235),(239),(246),(253),(256)

Sets B and C indicate that the change did not occur when the word-final consonant reflected a PMP velar; recall that PMP *q was a voiceless back velar stop (Blust 1991c); hence the forms in set C were excluded (after dialect split the vowel diphthongized in PLM but not K&R (3.2.3.1). Sets D and E indicate that monosyllables and `oxytones’ regularly escaped the change, for reasons of the accent. Thus, given the Malay-type pattern at the time the change occurred, the etyma in set E were accented on the ultimate vowel in pre-Rejang; therefore the change did not apply. As for set D, the change did not affect monosyllables, which lack metrical (word-level) accent (4.2). Note that t«baN falls in two sets: set B by virtue of the final consonant and set E by virtue of the penult vowel.

2.4.6.2 Interlude: Rule Order vs. Rule Complementation

McGinn (1997:71) argued that syllable-reduction rules producing monosyllables (e.g. Kebanagung dan from PMP *daqan) and oxytones (e.g. Kebanagung t«lan

from *tuqelaN) must be ordered before the merger of PMP *-aC and *-eC as PR *-«C. The Rawas data support this analysis without exception. Even more interestingly, the partial merger in question was complemented by other changes discussed in 3.2.1 and 4.2.2. Notably, the vowel assimilation process that produced PMP *qutek > Kebanagung *otok ‘brain’ was restricted to etyma ending with reflexes of PMP and PR velars *k and *j in all dialects except Rawas, which underwent a different set of changes. The Rawas evidence is conclusive that harmonization of PMP *qutek > Musi oto? occurred later than *-aC > *-«C, and therefore (as argued on independent grounds in earlier work), rule ordering (‘bleeding order’) is not a possible explanation for the failure of e.g. PMP *bulat > PR *bul«t ‘round’ and PMP *quzan > PR *uj«n ‘rain’ to become harmonized as unattested bolot** and ojon** in Kebanagung, Musi, Lebong and Pesisir. This argument is developed fully in 4.2.2.

2.4.6.3 Second Stress Shift and Vowel Harmony

Some GCs contrast solely in terms of whether the final syllable is open or closed. As recognized by Blust (1984), PMP vowel pairs *a-i and *a-u evolved differently in CVCV and CVCVC canons. See examples (5)-(6) of Table 3 and 2.4.1 changes (3b) and (4). In CVCV morphs the PMP vowel pairs *a-u and *a-i became PR *u-u and *i-i prior to diphthongizing the final vowels (see 3.2.4 below). Schematically:

2nd Stress Shift -V:-V- > -V-V:-

*a Harmonization a-u: > u-u: *sapu: > *supu: 'broom'

a-i: > i-i: *tali: > *tili: 'rope'

Diphthongization (de-Harmonization) *supu: > supaw 'broom' (P&L)

*tili: > tilay 'rope' (P&L)

*supu: > sup«w (MKR)

*tili: > til«y (MKR)

By contrast, in CVCVC morphs the same PMP vowel pairs became PR *o-o and *ä-ä, respectively, as shown in (7)-(8) of Table 3 and 2.4.1 (5) and (6). Schematically:

2nd Stress Shift -V:-V- > -V-V:-

*a shift/backing *-aCu:C > *-Cu:C *manu:k > *mnu:k `chicken'

*a shift/fronting *-aCi:C > *-ä:CiC *laNi:t > *läNi:t 'sky'

Harmonization CCu:C > CoCo:C *mnu:k > mono:k 'chicken'

CäCiC > CäCäC *läNi:t > läNä:t 'sky'

2.4.6.4 Dissimilation Without Deharmonization

Two rather similar rules called *u Lowering and *« Backing in McGinn (1997) introduced further instances of PR mid back *o. Both involved dissimilation; however, unlike later cases of dissimilation, the outcomes satisfied vowel harmony System A (see 2.4.6).

PMP pre-Rej

*u Lowering: *-i:CuC[+velar] > *-iCC *ikuR *i:koR 'tail'

PMP PR

*« Backing: *-«-«:C[+velar] > «-oC *tektek *t«to:k 'chop'

*u Lowering was conditioned by the Malay-type stress pattern; see 2.4.6

*« Backing presumably applied later because it affected stressed schwa. Evidence for *« Backing is shown in Table 10[17]. Note how *« Backing affected set A but not B below[18].

Table 10: Evidence for *«-Backing

PMP Pre-Rej PR PLM Keban. Rawas Gloss

A *deNeR d«No:R t«Noa t«Noa n.c. hear

*pegeN p«go:N goN goN goN hold

*tektek *t«t«k t«to:k t«to? t«tok t«tok chop, hack

*wahiR *w«y«R>w«yoR bio:l bioa bioa biol water

B *peRes p«r«:s p«?«s n.c. n.c. squeeze

*genep g«n«:p g«n«p g«n«p g«n«p complete

*gilap *g«l«p g«l«:p g«l«p g«l«p n.c. flash

*bener *b«n«:r b«n«:a b«n«a b«n«h b«n«a true

*tawed *taw«:r taw«:a taw«a taw«h taw«a haggle

*libeR *lib«:r lib«:a lib«a lib«h lib«a wide

Given the vowel-pair PMP *e-e (both schwa), *«-Backing changed (backed) the stressed member when followed a PMP velar ( PMP *R was presumably velar). The velarity condition explains the failure of the change to affect the forms in set B.

3. From Proto-Rejang to Contemporary Rawas

Much of Rawas' linguistic history has already been accounted for in the previous sections; this is inevitable given the methodological convenience of illustrating our reconstructed PR using Rawas evidence wherever possible (2.0). Moreover, apart from minor advancements noted in 4.1 below, the history of the other dialects is well known from previous studies (Blust 1984; McGinn 1997; 4.2 below). What remains to be accounted for are just the special features of Rawas, that is, the systematic exceptions to patterns of continuity and change in the other dialects.

The following changes occurred in Rawas (and only Rawas) after dialect split.

a) Diphthongization of final *i and *u in grammatical function words (3.3;4.2).

b) Change of PMP/PR *-j > t 'bleeding' *u-« Harmonization (3.2.1).

c) Retention of PR *-l 'bleeding' diphthongization of the tautosyllabic vowel (3.2.3.1).

In addition, Rawas shares the following with Kebanagung.

d) Borrowing (from Malay) of word-final -h (replacing PR *-? from PMP *-q)

accompanied by systematic failure of tautosyllabic vowels to diphthongize

(3.2.3.1).

These five developments are described under two topics: harmonization of the PMP/PR sequence *uCe in all dialects except Rawas, and diphthongization of PMP/PR final vowels. These topics are definitive of the Rejang language typologically, and are the major sources of dialect diversity. In particular, as noted in 1.0, Rawas' conservatism plays a major role in the description. To properly interpret some changes in the other dialects, it is always helpful--and sometimes necessary--to know what happened in Rawas.

3.1 Summary of Proto-Rejang Outcomes in Rawas

For Rawas diphthongs from PR diphthongs, see 2.2; for Rawas consonants from PR consonants, see (c)-(e) above and 2.3. The remainder of this section is devoted to the Rawas reflexes of PR vowels. The seven vowels of PR are reflected as 17 regular outcomes in Rawas, resulting in seven vowels and eight (new) diphthongs, summarized in Table 11.

The following vocalic developments in Rawas are the unexplained residues of the analysis presented in this paper.

1) Rawas pokot 'fish trap' (=Keb puk«t); expected puk«t from PR *puk«t; see

3.2.2 for discussion. Other Rawas words showing vowel harmony unattested in

the other dialects (and without clear conditions) are PR *kidek >

Rawas kede? 'evil'; PR *k«?iN > ki?iN 'dry', PR *b«?u? > bu?u? 'ape'.

2) Rawas mouy 'crocodile (=Keb bua«y); expected buuy** from PR *buuy.

3) Malay Influence may account for the following irregular vocalic outcomes in

Rawas.

a) Rawas tan« 'sign' (cf. PMP *tanda ‘sign’); expected Rawas

tanaw** = Keb tano). See Table 13. Probable source: Besemah Malay.

b) Rawas kait 'fish hook'; also Kebanagung kait (cf. PMP *kawit, PR *käwät

‘hook’); expected Rawas käwät** = Pesisir kewet).

c) All dialects including Rawas tokot 'staff, cane' (cf. PMP *tuNked

'staff, cane'); expected tuk«t** (all dialects). See 3.2.2 for discussion.

3.2 Patterns of Vocalic Change in Rawas

After dialect split, Gestalt Conditions (GC--see 1.5) continued to play a role in vocalic change in Rawas. In particular, all vocalic changes were conditioned by the contemporary accent. It is important to bear in mind that the accent, being predictable on the final syllable of the word, does not appear in the phonemic representations. See 1.3.3.

As mentioned in 3.0, two classes of vocalic change having unique effects in Rawas were (i) diphthongization of vowels (which affected each dialect in different ways); and (ii) harmonization of the PMP/PR vowel-pair *u-« (which affected four of five dialects--all except Rawas). Let us begin with the latter, which is particularly striking for the way in which the Rawas evidence sheds light on the other dialects.

Table 11. Regular Development of Vowels and Diphthongs

in Rawas

PR *a (2) CHANGE SEC. RAWAS DEVELOPMENT KEBAN. COGNATE

1) *-a > aw 3.2.3 PR *t«ka > t«kaw 'come' (cf. K t«ko)

2) *a > a 2.4.1 PR *mati > mat«y 'eye' (cf. K mat«y)

PR *« (3)

1) *« > «a 3.2.3 PR *b«n«r > b«n«a ‘true’ (cf. K b«n«h)

2) *« > a 3.2.1 PR *ut«k > uta? 'brain' (cf. K otok)

3) *« > « 3.2.1 PR *pus«j > pus«t 'navel' (cf. K posog)

3.3.2 PR *it« > it« 'we (incl)' (cf. K it«)

PR *i (5)

1) *i > «y 3.2.3 PR *tili > til«y 'rope' (cf. K til«y)

2) *i > ia 3.2.3 PR *bibir > bibia ‘lip’ (cf. K bebea)

3) *i > ä 3.2.3 PR *pili? > päläh ‘choose’ (cf. K peleah)

4) *i > äy 3.3.2 PR *kimi > kämäy ‘1Ppl (excl)’ (cf. K keme)

5) *i > i -- PR *isi > is«y 'contents' (cf. K is«y)

PR *u (4)

1) *u > «w 3.2.3 PR *supu > sup«w 'broom' (cf. K sup«w)

2) *u > ua 3.2.3 PR *ulur > ulua ‘lower’ (cf. K oloa)

3) *u > o 3.2.3 PR *pulu? > poloh ‘ten’ (cf. K poloah)

4) *u > u -- PR *l«suN > l«suN 'mortar' (cf. K l«suN)

PR *o, *e, *ä (3)(all retentions)

1) *o > o PR *monok > mono? 'chicken' (cf. K monok)

2) *e > e PR *kidek > kede? 'rotten' (cf. K kidek)

3) *ä > ä PR *läNät > Nät 'sky' (cf. K leNet)

PR *äpän > äpän ‘tooth’ (cf. K epen)

3.2.1 Harmonization of PMP Vowel pairs *u-e and *i-e

We return next to examples (9)-(10) of Table 3 (1.5). A complete display of the relevant data is provided in Table 12, especially sets A and D.

Table 12: Harmonization of PMP/PR *i-« and *u-«

PMP PR PLM Keban. Rawas Gloss

A *ipen *äpän epen epen äpän tooth

*isep *äsäp esep esep äsäp suck

*hiket *äkät eket eket äkät to tie

*um-inem *min«m menem menem n.c. drink

B *libeR *lib«r lib«a lib«h lib«a wide

*pinzem *piñ«m iñ«m iñ«m iñ«m borrow

*lalej *dal«j dal«k dal«g dal«t housefly

*gilap *gil«p>*g«l«p g«l«p g«l«p n.c. flash

C. *kizep *kij«p s«-kij«p k«nd«rij«p k«dip blink

*kilat -- g«l«p smitoa kil«t lightning

*tikam *tujaq tik«m tujah tujah to stab

D *pusej *pus«j posok posog pus«t navel

*qulej *ul«j olok olog ul«t maggot

*qutek *ut«k oto? otok utak brain

E *puket *puk«t puk«t n.c. pokot dragnet

*bulat *bul«t bul«t bul«t bul«t round

*quzan *uz«n uj«n uj«n uj«n rain

*buhek *buk bu? buk bu? head hair

F *tuNked *tokot tokot tokot tokot staff, cane

As indicated in sets A and D of Table 12, the two harmonization processes operated in parallel in all dialects except Rawas, where *u-« failed to harmonize.[19] This fact has clear implications for relative ordering of the two processes.

a) Before dialect split: As shown in sets A-C in Table 12, the vowels in PMP/PR vowel-pair *i-« underwent mutual assimilation, becoming PR *ä-ä, reflected as Rawas ä-ä and PLMK e-e, when the specific GC was met. The following schematizes the harmonization process.

PMP *-iCeC[-velar] > PR *-äCä:C[-velar]

:where [-velar] = reflexes of PMP consonants except velars

b) After dialect split: As shown in sets D-E of Table 12, in all dialects except Rawas the vowels in PMP/PR vowel-pair *u-« underwent mutual assimilation, becoming PR *o-o, when the specific GC was met. The following schematizes the harmonization process.

PR *-uC«:C[+velar] > -oCo:C[+velar]

:where [+velar] = reflexes of PMP velars and *-R (or PR *-r)

Set B of Table 12 illustrates the claim that the final [+velar] consonant PMP *-R or PR *-r (both [+velar]) blocked harmonization of *i-«. Thus PMP/PR *lib«R did not undergo vowel harmonization because the etymon ended with *-R (= [+velar]). Next, compare the outcomes for 'drink' in set A and 'borrow' in set B. Apparently *i-« harmonization was blocked by intervening consonant clusters (which eventually became ‘barred nasals' (Coady and McGinn 1983)): thus iñ«m (all dialects) from PMP *p-inzem resisted harmonization in conformity with the rule (which allows for -C- but not -CC- between harmonizing vowels). Next, PR *dal«j from PMP *lalej failed to harmonize because it had the wrong penult vowel (*a rather than *u or *i).

Set D of Table 12 illustrates the parallel effects with respect to *u-« harmonization in the harmonizing dialects (see 3.2.2). Notice that Rawas offers indirect evidence in support McGinn (1997)'s analysis of *u-« Harmonization. In particular, Rawas' non-participation in *u-« > o-o harmony is predicted given the fact that word-final PMP/PR *-j changed to Rawas -t. This change altered a crucial part of the Gestalt; in particular, it changed the final consonant from [+velar] to [-velar], in effect 'bleeding' the rule. By contrast, in the harmonizing dialects, the PMP/PR vowel-pair *u-« underwent mutual assimilation as illustrated in sets D-F of Table 12. Set D illustrates the change; set E illustrates the blocking effect of final non-velar -C.

Here as elsewhere in Rejang historical phonology, systematic exceptions seem to outnumber the forms undergoing a regular change. Thus the outcome for ‘head hair’ has a straightforward explanation given that syllable reductions preceded all harmonization rules (McGinn 1997, 1999, 2000); thus PMP *buhek was reduced to PR *buk before the harmonization schema could apply (thus bok** is unattested). See 3.5.1 for discussion of the role played by rule ordering in the analysis. Another interesting case is PMP *gilap 'flash' > g«l«p in most dialects. This form must have been *gil«p in pre-Rejang, and if so, it should have become unattested gelep** by *i-« Harmonization. The fact that it did not is explained by an earlier rule changing penult *i > « (cf. 3.2.1) which effectively 'bled' harmonization by altering relevant segments, in effect altering the Gestalt. Therefore g«l«p may be regular. Finally, I assume that the exceptional outcomes in set C of Table 12 were due borrowing or analogy. In particular, the PLM words for 'stab' and the Rawas word for ‘lightning’ appear to be partly regularized borrowings from Malay tikam and kilat; and the various outcomes for 'blink' may have been influenced by morphology (but if so the mechanism is unclear).

3.2.2 Rawas pokot and tokot

The analysis is not without its problems stemming from the Rawas lexicon, however. Consider the Rawas outcomes tokot ‘staff, cane’ and pokot ‘fish trap’ (Table 12 sets E and F). The anticipated outcomes are unattested tuk«t** and puk«t** parallel to Rawas bul«t 'round' and uj«n 'rain'. Recall that the GC governing harmonization of

the Gestalt -uC«:C[+velar] predicts (falsely) that PR *puk«:t should not harmonize, a prediction that is upheld by puk«t in all dialects except Rawas.

McGinn (1997:85) accounted for PKM puk«t as regular and for tokot as a borrowing from Malay tongkat which was then regularized in conformity with contemporary (synchronic) morpheme structure restrictions prohibiting (a) voiceless intervocalic nasal clusters and (b) penultimate mid-vowels paired with any vowel except the selfsame ultimate vowels (cf. Malay topi = Rejang topong ‘western style hat’) and many other examples. See 2.1.2. Perhaps Rawas pokot as well can be discounted as a Rawas borrowing from Malay pukat.[20]

3.2.3 Contemporary Diphthongs From Proto-Rejang Vowels

Table 13 shows every known diphthong-type derivable from a simple vowel in contemporary Rejang. The diversity of the dialects is greatest in the manner in which diphthongs developed from vowels. This claim is amply illustrated in Table 13, and underlies two related claims made in this paper. (i) The majority of Rejang diphthongs developed after dialect split. (ii) The only diphthongs in PR were derived from PMP diphthongs (2.2).

As Table 13 shows, Rawas again proves the exception to broad tendencies in the post-split vocalic development of the five dialects. Four observations are especially noteworthy. (i) In all dialects except Rawas, PR *-a regularly became -o corresponding to aw in Rawas (set A of Table 13). (ii) Whereas PR *-i and *-u regularly diphthongized in all dialects (set B), only in Rawas did the process become generalized to include pronouns and other clitics (see 5-6 and 8-9 pf Table 13 and 3.3 below). (iii) In all dialects except Rawas, word-final PR *-l disappeared and PR tautosyllabic *o and diphthongized to oa and ea respectively, whereas in Rawas, PR word-final *l was retained and the vowels did not diphthongize (see 15-17 of Table 13 and 3.2.3.1). (iv) In all dialects except Rawas and Kebanagung, PR *-? (from PMP *-q) was retained as -? and tautosyllabic high vowels *u and *i diphthongized in diverse ways, whereas in Rawas and Kebanagung, PR *-? was replaced by a borrowed phoneme -h (2.3.2.1) and the adjacent high vowels did not diphthongize, at least not in Rawas (they diphthongized anyway in Kebanagung). See 22-25 of Table 13 and 3.2.3.1.

The issues that require further discussion are taken up in the next few subsections, namely: (i) the role of final consonants in diphthongization; (ii) secondary harmonization, and (iii) diphthongization of final vowels in grammatical function words (which is a problem not for Rawas but for the other dialects).

3.2.3.1 Diphthongization of *-VC where *-C = PR *-r ,*-l, *-?

Consider set D of Table 13. An important generalization is that loss of PR *-l or *-r is associated with diphthongization of the tautosyllabic vowel. This holds for all dialects, and suggests that consonantal changes preceded diphthongization in each case. For example, in Rawas three cases of loss of PR *-r are associated with three diphthongs in set D, namely, PR *-«r became «a; PR *-ir became ia, and PR *ur became ua. The same principle accounts for Kebanagung's two diphthongs in set D compared to three in Musi and five in Pesisir and Lebong. The analysis depends heavily on our reconstruction of word-final PR *-r and *-l. Recall that PR *-r derives from two PMP sources: *-r and*-R; likewise PR *-l derives from two PMP sources: *-R and *-l. See 2.3.2.1.

3.2.3.2 Secondary Harmonization

The Rawas outcomes for 'white' and 'choose' are especially interesting with respect to secondary harmonization of the penult vowels. The simplest solution that is plausible phonetically is to assume that the end-rhymes PR *-i? and *-u? developed differently in Rawas (whereas they developed in parallel in the other dialects). In particular, PR *pulu? 'ten' became Rawas poloh by the shortest possible route via intermediate *puluh and *puloh. By contrast, PR *ili? 'choose' became äläh by first diphthongizing the end-rhyme, yielding *ilia? similar to PR *bibir > Rawas bibia. After diphthongization, intermediate *ilia? underwent the remainder of its derivation by the shortest route, namely via intermediate *iliah > iläh > äläh. The advantage of early diphthongization is that it motivates ä from PR *i in a dialect with an established phoneme e (albeit from unknown sources). The alternative absolute shortest-route derivation may be possible but seems less plausible phonetically. Another argument for this solution is that a similar disjunction is observed in the case of *u-« harmonization and *i-« harmonization (previous section directly above). In all dialects except Rawas these two vowel pairs harmonized in parallel, becoming o-o and e-e respectively; but in Rawas, *i-« harmonized (as ä-ä) but *u-« did not harmonize at all. A third argument concerns the central step in the derivation of PR *-i?, namely, coalescence of peak and coda of the derived diphthong *-iah as *-äh. A partial precedent for coalescence may be found in the fact that intervocalically

Table 13. Rejang Diphthongs Reflecting PMP Vowels

PMP pre-R PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas GLOSS

A 1. *teka *t«ka: t«ko t«ko t«ko t«ko t«kaw come

Appendix: (56),(106),(128),(190) (218),(233); cf. also (224)

B 2 *isi *isi: isay isay is«y is«y is«y contents

Appendix: (38),(40),(89),(112),(132), (146),(159),(245), (258)

3. *mata>mat« *mati: matay matay mat«y mat«y mat«y eye

Appendix: (49),(72),(130),(238)

4. *duha>du« *dui: duay duay du«y dui du«y two

Appendix: (49),(72),(238)

5. *si-ia *si si si si si s«y 3sg/pl

6. *kami *kimi keme keme keme keme kämäy 1pl(excl)

C 7 *qulu *ulu: ulaw ulaw ul«w ul«w ul«w head

Appendix: (29),(40),(52),(65),(172),(203),(206),(220),(248)

8. *aku *uku uku uku uku uku uk«w 1sg

9. *kamu *kumu kumu kumu kumu kumu kum«w 2(honor)

D

10. *bibiR *bibir bibia bibia bebea n.c. bibia lips

11. *hiliR *ilir n.c. n.c. elea ilih n.c. upstream

12. *huluR *ulur ulua ulua oloa uluh ulua to lower

13. *qapuR *upur upua upua opoa k-opoh upua chalk

14. *niuR *niol nioa nioa nioa nioa niol coconut

15. *dapuR *dopol dopoa dopoa dopoa dopoa dopol hearth

16. *kawil *käwäl kewea kewea kewea kewea n.c. fishhook

17. -- *käkäl kekea kekea kekea kekea käkäl foot

18. -- *k«bol k«boa k«boa k«boa k«boa k«bol thick

Appendix: (35),(36),(71),(84),(101),(102),(104),(157), (221),(223),

(228) (249),(250)

19. *bener *b«n«r b«n«a b«n«a b«n«a b«n«h b«n«a true

20. *tawed *taw«r taw«a taw«a taw«a taw«h taw«a haggle

21. *libeR *lib«r lib«a lib«a lib«a lib«h lib«a wide

Appendix: (30),(36),(131),(186) (215)

22. *hasaq *asa? as«a? as«a? as«a? asah asah sharpen

Appendix: (7),(18),(43),(57),(69), (123),(136),(150),(163),(173),(193),(234)

23. *taneq *tana? tan«a? tan«a? tan«a? tanah tanah earth

24. *putiq *puti? putia? putia? putea? puteah putäh white

Appendix: (85),(168),(174),(187)

25. *p«nuq *p«nu? p«nua? p«nua? p«noa? p«noah p«noh full

Appendix: (174),(178),(244),(255)


PR *-ai- regularly coalesced as ä in Rawas, e.g. PMP *paqit > PR *pait > Rawas pät. This analysis entails the derivations given in Table 14.

It is hardly surprising to discover that another GC governed secondary harmonization in Rawas, Kebanagung and Musi. Thus the intermediate vowel pairs *i-ea (Musi *ilea?) and *u-oa (Kebanagung *puloah) did not harmonize independently of surrounding consonants; rather, secondary harmonization of the penult vowel was conditioned by the end-rhyme, i.e. not only the diphthong but the tautosyllabic consonant. Thus PR *kidek 'rotten' showing final vowel-pair *i-e and word-final *-k did not trigger harmonization of Musi kide? and Kebanagung kidek (Musi kede?** and Kebanagung kedek** are unattested). And even Rawas kede? from PR *kidek, which did undergo late harmonization of the penult vowel (2.4.5), cannot be generalized together with äläh for obvious reasons. Instead, to account for Rawas äläh from PR *ili? it is plain that the syllables harmonized only when the Gestalt included just the right end-rhyme.

3.3 Personal Pronouns

The last correspondence sets to be considered in this section concern the personal pronouns, including (5), (6), (8), and (9) of Table 13, where again the uniqueness of Rawas is on display. The pronouns present a problem pointed out by Blust (1984:441; cf. McGinn (1997:75f). For reasons discussed in 4.2.1, pronouns resisted diphthongization of PR *-i and *-u in all dialects except Rawas. This issue clearly does not concern Rawas, at least not directly; nonetheless, the Rawas evidence is extremely important for the light it sheds on Proto-Rejang and the histories of the other dialects.

As in many Western Austronesian languages, Rejang personal pronouns come in a long form (roughly, subject) and a short-form (non-subject).[21] Table 15 displays the correspondences among pronouns in the five dialects. Table 16 gives the Rawas pronouns in relation to the reconstructed protolanguages PMP and PR.

Problems of lexical replacement notwithstanding (3.3.2 below), it is clear that Rawas pronouns underwent the regular diphthongization rules affecting PR *-i and

TABLE 14. Effects of Secondary Harmonization

a) without secondary harmonization

PMP *putiq ‘white’

PR *puti?

P&L *puti? > putia?

Musi *puti? > *pute? > putea?

Keban *puti? > *putih > *puteh > puteah

Rawas *puti? > *putia? > putiah > *putäh

PMP *p«nuq ‘full’

PR *p«nu?

P&L *p«nu? > p«nua?

Musi *p«nu? > p«nua? > p«noa?

Keban *p«nu? > *p«nuh > *p«nuah > p«noah

Rawas *p«nu? > *p«nuh > p«noh

b) with secondary harmonization

PMP *piliq ‘choose’

PR *ili?

P&L *ili? > ilia?

Musi *ili? > *ilia? > *ilea? > elea?

Keban *ili? > *ilih > *iliah > *ileah > eleah

Rawas *ili? > *ilia? *iliah > iläh > äläh

PMP *puluq ‘ten’

PR *pulu?

P&L *pulu? > pulua?

Musi *pulu? > *pulo? > poloa?

Keban *pulu? > *puluh > puluah > puloah > poloah

Rawas *pulu? > *puluh > puloh > poloh

XZThis table is badly placedXZ


*-u. Equally important, as Table 17 below demonstrates, in Rawas other grammatical classes behaved similarly, again in contrast to the other dialects, where function words generally (including pronouns) escaped diphthongization of *-i and *-u (see 4.2).

What is patently clear is that in Rawas, the function words developed diphthongs by the same rules as other grammatical classes. More generally, since all diphthongization rules in all dialects occurred after dialect split, it follows that the explanation for the regular developments in Rawas may not apply in other dialects, especially in those dialects where putative regularities have been observed. See 4.2.

3.3.2 Residual Problems

1) The most pressing problem is to account for the failure of diphthongization in pronouns and other function words in all dialects except Rawas. This problem is addressed in section 4.2.

2) Rawas kämäy from PR *kimi is unexpected, as is keme in the other dialects (expected kim«y** in Rawas and kimi** in PLMK). See 4.2.1 for discussion. The existential verb Rawas adäy = PLMK ade likewise shows the expected regular correspondence äy = e, e.g. Rawas äpän = PLMK epen 'tooth'. However, there is insufficient data to determine whether kämäy represents a regular development based on pre-Rawas *kim«y from PR *kimi.

Table 15. Contemporary Personal Pronouns

subject-object agent-possessive subject-object agent-possessive

PLMK Singular Rawas __

1Pers uku ku uk«w k«w

2Pers ko nu kab«n kab«n

2Pers (hon) kumu kumu kum«w kum«w

3Pers si n« s«y n«

Plural

1P(incl) it« t« it« t«

1P(excl) keme keme kämäy kämäy

2Pers udi udi gal«ygal«y gal«ygal«y

2Pers (hon) kumukumu kumukumu gal«ygal«y kum«w gal«ygal«y kum«w

3Pers si n« s«y n«

Table 16: Rawas Personal Pronouns and Their Etyma

PMP PR pre-Rawas Rawas Gloss

*aku *uku uk«w I

*kahu *ko kab«n you (sg)

*ni-hu *nu kab«n you (sg/poss)

*(ka)mu *kumu (hon) kum«w you (sg,hon)

*si-ia *si s«y s/he; they

*ita *it« it« we (inclusive)

*kami *kimi *kim«y kämäy we (exclusive)

*kamu *udi *kumukumu kum«wkum«w you (pl)

*si-iDa *si; tobo o s«y; tobo « they; that group

3) Two lexical replacements affecting 2nd Person pronouns remain mysterious: PLMK udi; and Rawas kab«n. Possibly Rawas kab«n is an internal borrowing based on kab«n ‘friend’ (cf. Ujan Mas Malay kaba (McGinn 1991:219)).

4) Another replacement is variable. Rawas si = s«y 'he/she/it; they' can be used in the singular or plural. When the context demands that the plurality be emphasized, the phrase tobo « 'that group' is common in Rawas (=tobo o in the other dialects).

Finally PR *k«bol (if from PMP *kapal) shows unexplained *b from *p and *o from *a.

PMP PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas Gloss

5) *ma-kapal *k«bol k«boa k«boa k«boa k«boa k«bol thick

4. Consequences of the Analysis

It was mentioned in section 2 of this paper that every feature of Proto-Rejang can be justified based on evidence from Rawas and one other dialect—either Pesisir or (most often) Kebanagung. As it happens, these are the only dialects that share a boundary with a dialect of Malay. Some consequences of this fact are discussed in 4.4.3 below. It was also mentioned that without Rawas the remaining dialects differ too little among themselves to offer much in the way of time depth. If those dialects were all linguists had to go on, it might be concluded that the Rejangs were relatively recent arrivals in Sumatra. The pioneering work of Blust (1984) on the Musi dialect, however, might suggest the opposite given the extremely high number of changes (over 100 according to McGinn 1999) separating Rejang-Musi from Proto-Austronesian. With the discovery of Rawas the possibilities have narrowed and start to become reconciled with the linguistic facts: the time depth is increased; the reconstruction of Proto-Rejang becomes feasible; and certain practical questions can be raised, such as: How long have the Rejangs been in Sumatra? Where did they come from? What language or language group is their closest linguistic relative? These and other questions are addressed below as we attempt to extract the most important consequences from the historical phonology presented in this paper.

Four topics will occupy us in this concluding section. All are concerned with the linguistic contributions of Rawas with respect to the goal of developing a valid and useful historical phonology for the Rejang language of Sumatra. First, analytical improvements are considered in relation to previous research (4.1 below). Second, empirical confirmations of earlier work are revisited in light of the new evidence from Rawas (4.2). Third, potential contributions of Rejang to the theory of sound change

Table 17: Rejang Function Words

PR Pesisir Lebong Musi Keban Rawas Gloss

*nak~taN di na? di na? di na? di nah di taN d«y (at) there

*apa~api api api api api apaw who

*adäy ade ade ade ade adäy there is/are

*ba ba ba ba ba ba emph. part.

*unu unu unu unu unu n.d. hesitation part.


are considered (4.3). Fourth, the usefulness of our reconstructed PR is considered in relation to certain practical questions pertaining to the origin and likely closest linguistic relatives of the Rejangs (4.4).

4.1 Analytical Advancements In Relation To Previous Work

Four claims made in earlier work by McGinn (1997, 1999, 2000) have been abandoned or modified here. Two concern laryngeals and two concern diphthongs.[22]

1) The opening statement in McGinn (1997) must be abandoned ("Every known Rejang dialect has a single laryngeal, namely, h or ?") because Rawas has both

-h (in word-final position only) and ? (word-medially as well as word-finally).

2) Pre-Rejang *h has been abandoned and replaced by Proto-Rejang *r in light of the limited distribution of Rawas h. This paper has reconstructed PR *r in all positions reflecting PMP *r, *R and *l; later Rawas and Kebanagung borrowed -h from Malay (cf. Blust 1992); and later still, Kebanagung substituted h for PR *r in all positions. A crucial assumption is that PR *r was [+velar]; thus PR *r plays the same role assigned to PMP/PR *R in McGinn (1997) with respect to the conditioning of vocalic changes. See 2.3.2.2.

3) Two metatheses suggested by McGinn (2000 Appendix 2) have been abandoned in light of the Rawas evidence. The problem was (and is) to account for Kebanagung ponoy 'dove' from PMP *punay; and also for Kebanagung ki«a 'wood' from PMP *kahiw. The metathesis idea collapsed in light of the fact that Rawas retains PMP diphthongs *uy and *iw unaltered. It is now straightforward to derive Rawas punuy and kiiw from PR *punuy and PR *kiiw, from which all other dialect outcomes follow as described in 2.1.3.

4) A final improvement derives straightforwardly from the fact that PMP *ey and *ew have been removed from the inventory of PMP diphthongs (Blust, personal communication); they have been collapsed with PMP *ay and *aw, respectively. This move simplifies the derivation of Rejang diphthongs from PMP diphthongs. (Rejang provided no evidence for PMP *ey and *ew.)

4.2 Empirical Confirmations

We turn next to consider a number of cases where the new dialect evidence from Rawas, although diverging from the other dialects, does so in such a way as to strengthen specific claims made in earlier work on the historical phonology of Rejang (i.e. before any Rawas data was available).

4.2.1. Rawas Pronouns and other Function Words

At first the Rawas function words appear to present counterexamples with respect to earlier analyses of RHP, but on closer inspection the problem vanishes in light of the claim that Rawas simply generalized regular diachronic rules by relaxing phonological conditions that held in PR and that continue to hold in the other dialects.

As pointed out in 3.3, diphthongization of word-final PR *-i and *-u was not limited to content words in Rawas (as it was in the other dialects), but extended to all morpheme classes equally. When properly understood as post-split effects, the Rawas facts are consistent with the analysis of the other dialects in earlier work. As pointed out in my earlier paper:

"The analysis is consistent with the striking fact that Rejang function words and content words differ in canonical shape. With few exceptions (notably o), content words almost always end with a diphthong if not a consonant, whereas function words almost always end with vowels; and only function words have been observed ending with schwa. Thus ... the synchronic generalization is consistent with the claim of regularity. ...Although their histories diverged (=content words, function words), the divergence had a phonetic basis, and in particular, all reflexes of PMP vowels in the pronouns were regular." (McGinn 1997:77)

What is unique about Rawas, then, is that the conditions changed (generalized). Two closely-related arguments support this claim. First, all diphthongization of PR vowels occurred after dialect split. In all dialects except Rawas, those diphthongization rules were conditioned by the accent, which falls on the last syllable of content words; hence only stressed vowels diphthongized. Therefore, there is a ready explanation why pronouns and other function words were exempted from diphthongization rules in PLMK; as clitics, function words bear no inherent (word-level) stress pattern. They may receive stress within the domain of the sentence (intonation) but not at the word (lexical) level, which is presumably the proper domain for the study of sound change. It follows that there are phonetic grounds for the claim that pronouns and other function words were systematically exempted from rules applying to stressed vowels in Pesisir, Lebong, Musi, and Kebanagung. The second argument concerns an interesting bit if new information which is highly welcome, namely, Rawas' 1P(excl) pronoun kämäy from PR *kimi and PMP *kami. This outcome supports the reconstruction of PR *kimi, thus completely regularizing the pronouns at the level of PR.

The conclusion I draw from these two arguments is that the Rawas facts are consistent in every important detail with earlier claims about the pronouns of pre-Rejang (now Proto-Rejang).

4.2.2 Rawas As `Best Witness' For Rejang Historical Phonology

Most of this paper has focused on the uniqueness of Rawas in relation to the other dialects. In this section, the focus shifts to the conformity of Rawas, especially those specific cases where it is clear that Rawas directly confirms earlier work on the historical phonology of Rejang. The following are the major aspects of Rejang historical phonology as presented by McGinn (1997, 2000). All are supported by the Rawas evidence.

1) Pre-Rejang underwent two accent shifts: (a) 1st stress shift to Malay-type pattern regularly followed by neutralization of unstressed PMP *a except before velars, e.g. PMP *mata > pre-Rejang *ma:t« `eye'; PMP *taNan > pre-Rejang *ta:N«n `hand'. (b) 2nd Stress Shift to contemporary Rejang pattern, followed by V-V harmonization of certain vowel pairs and then diphthongization affecting certain word-final syllables, e.g. PMP *laNit > pre-Rejang *laNi:t > PR *läNät 'sky' and *isi > *isi: > Rawas is«y 'contents'.

2) As expected in accord with (1), unstressed vowels weakened (deleted, neutralized, or harmonized), and stressed vowels became strengthened (diphthongized, de-neutralized).

3) No fewer than ten vocalic shifts were conditioned by complex morph-shape conditions labeled GCs (hereafter GC) rather than merely locally by strictly adjacent segments. See 4.3.2. A partial list of GCs is provided in Table 3.

4) Two of the Gestalt Conditions can be generalized and captured in a single formula. The essential features of this claim, which may have important consequences for subgrouping,[23] are partially repeated here.

Table 18: Raising of PMP *a

PMP pre-Rej PR Rawas Gloss

*a > « /V(C[-velar]) _ # *duha *du:« *dui: du«y two

| *mata *ma:t« *mati: mat«y eye

[-stress] *kita *it« *it« it« 1Ppl(incl)

*ni-a *ni-« *n« n« 3Psg

*taNan *ta:N«n *taN«:n taN«n hand

*anak *a:nak *ana:k anak child

*teka *t«ka: *t«ka: t«kaw come

*timba *ti:ma *tima: timaw pail

*daqan *dan *dan dan branch

The Rawas outcomes are perfectly consistent with the earlier analysis presented in McGinn (1997), before anything was known about this dialect.

5) Rule Order vs. Rule Complementation (Revisited)[24]

As mentioned in 2.4.6.2, two traditional tools of the Comparative Method are rule order and rule complementation. However, when two (or more) rules are in complementary distribution all claims about (external) rule order become obviated. The following illustrates the point with two conditioned changes presented in McGinn (1997).

1) PMP *qut«k > Musi oto? 'brain' :only when *-C = [+velar]

PMP *lib«R > Musi lib«a 'wide'

2) PMP *bulan > Musi bul«n 'moon' (not bolon**) :only when *-C = [-velar]

PMP *anak > Musi ana? 'child'

Blust (1984) recognized that rule (2) regularly failed to apply before a velar consonant, but the fact that rule (1) applied only before velars was overlooked. The Musi facts only become clear in light of dialect evidence from Kebanagung, where PMP *-k is preserved as -k and *-j becomes -g (McGinn 1997:68). Further, Blust assumed (a) that Musi puk«t was an unexplained exception, and (b) that change (1) must have preceded change (2) in the linear ordering (hence in relative chronology) in order to explain why Musi *bul«n 'moon' failed regularly to become bolon**. McGinn (1997) proposed an alternative analysis, namely, that the environments were in complementary distribution with respect to the binary feature [ +velar ] associated with the word-ending consonants. If so, then two consequences followed: (a) Musi puk«t was regular because the etymon ended with an non-velar consonant; and (b) linear ordering--crucially--was unnecessary to account for the failure of e.g. bul«n to become bolon** in Musi.

The Rawas evidence mentioned in 3.2.1 is relevant for this argument. Rawas underwent rule (2) but not rule (1). This fact permits an improvement of McGinn's analysis by actually giving away the correct ordering, namely: change (2) preceded change (1). This is inescapable since (2) affected all dialects equally (hence may be reconstructed for PR) whereas (1) occurred after dialect split everywhere except in Rawas (where a different set of changes occurred). Also supported is the claim that changes (1) and (2) were in complementary distribution in the harmonizing dialects, exactly as proposed by McGinn (1997).

4.3 Theoretical Contributions of Rejang Historical Phonology

The data in this paper may be of theoretical interest with respect to the following three questions. (i) What is the proper domain for the assignment of word-level stress rules in Rejang (and other languages)? (ii) What is the proper relationship between stress rules and syllabification rules in Rejang (and other languages)? [25] (iii) What is the status of Gestalt Conditions in the historical phonology of Rejang (and other languages)?

4.3.1 Stress and Syllabification

As mentioned in the Introduction to the paper, Rejang should be of interest to linguists if only because of its rich array of diphthongs, especially the ones that arose by regular changes from PMP vowels (3.2.3). My remarks here are limited to this set of innovating diphthongs. Three points bear repeating in this context. (a) Since these diphthongs affected stressed vowels, they must have arisen after the stress had shifted to the final syllable. (b) Since the innovating diphthongs differ from dialect to dialect, they must have arisen after dialect split (probably influenced by areal pressures (4.4.3)). (c) Since last-syllable stress is shared by all dialects, the stress assignment rule must be older than the innovating diphthongs.

The theoretical point to be made here is that the stress assignment rule at whatever level one examines seems to depend crucially on prior recognition of the segmental structure of words, and in particular, on the proper identification of syllabic and non-syllabic vowels. [26] Once this is accomplished, assigning stress to “the last nonsyllabic vowel of the word” is straightforward: forms like tidoa are disyllabic once non-syllabic [a9] is recognized as the coda of a breaking diphthong, and forms like oa? are monosyllables.

The analysis not only simplifies the stress assignment rule for Rejang, it also supports the empirical claim of Bromberger and Halle (1989) concerning the derivational relationship between syllabification rules and stress assignment rules in the theory of (synchronic) phonology. We can add the point that ontogeny repeats phylogeny in the Rejang case. In PR the stress assignment rule arose before the breaking diphthongs.

4.3.2 Word-Level Stress Is 'Metrical'

In the historical phonology of Rejang, the phenomenon of multiple reflexes of PMP last-syllable *a have been explained in terms of the word-level stress pattern operating at different times (2.4.6.1) This explanation depends on the assumption that word-level stress (also called accent) is assigned 'metrically' within a disyllabic domain called a 'foot'. Applied to disyllabic word bases, this theory states that stress, whether predictable or contrastive, must be assigned to either the penultimate vowel or the ultimate vowel. An important corollary is that monosyllables cannot bear metrical word-stress, by definition since stress differentiates between the members of a pair of vowels within a specified domain.

This theory has an important consequence for Rejang historical phonology, namely, it explains why monosyllables did not participate in the changes affecting PMP last-syllable *a. The relevant outcomes include both original monosyllables such as (e.g. PMP *ba 'interrogative particle' > PR *ba (not b«**) 'emphatic particle'; and also derived monosyllables (e.g. PMP *hekan > pre-Rejang *kan > PR *kan 'fish' (not k«n**) beside PMP *taNan > PR *taN«n 'hand'. See 2.4.6.1.

4.3.3 Gestalt Conditions (GC) and the Regularity Hypothesis (RH)

This section seeks theoretical support for the use of GCs in the historical phonology of Rejang, and discusses their potential contribution to the theory of sound change. Two ideas from Kiparsky (1988) guide the discussion.

1) The Exceptionless Hypothesis (EH) must be distinguished from the Regularity Hypothesis (RH) (Kiparsky 1988:390). The EH is wedded to phonetic mechanisms that apply blindly and randomly (see 1.4 of this paper). The RH is conceptually simpler, more open-ended, and less theory-dependent than the EH. The RH says that sound changes tend overwhelmingly to be regular, for whatever the reason; and that depending upon the theory one adopts, further restrictions on the scope and limits of sound change can and must be determined. For example, in Kiparsky's theory, regular sound changes operate over the domain of phonological structures in the lexicon.

2) Sound change is structure-dependent (Kiparsky 1988:390). Furthermore, structure arises from implicational universals and from individual grammars.

In this subsection we attempt to apply these theoretical points to justify the existence and use of GCs in RHP. Our major empirical claim is that GCs are structures that refer to one or all of the following: the accent, the quality of the penult vowel, and the nature of the word-final consonant (if present). Once recognized, they play a major role because they induce regularity throughout in RHP (1.4 and Table 3). Virtually all vocalic changes in Rejang were conditioned by GCs operating on the level of the disyllabic base (prosodic foot), and resulted in a high degree of lexical vowel harmony in pre-Rejang and Proto-Rejang, which is largely, but not entirely, reflected in the contemporary dialects. The most dramatic demonstration of this point is found in Rawas' non-participation in *u-« > o-o harmony (3.2.1). This fact was explained in terms of another fact unique to Rawas, namely, the change of word-final PMP/PR *-j > Rawas -t. This change altered the Gestalt; in particular, the final consonant changed from [+velar] to [-velar], in effect 'bleeding' the harmonization process exactly as predicted by the form of the rule as proposed in McGinn (1997) to account for the harmonization facts in Musi, Pesisir and Kebanagung.

The conclusion I draw is as follows. To the extent that GCs contribute to the overall regularity of RHP they are consistent with the demands of RH. However, to the extent that they cannot be construed as purely phonetic conditions, their existence cannot be justified in terms of EH. It follows that GCs lie outside the bounds of the neogrammarian theory of sound change (EH). Expressed in more general terms, the right place to seek the motivation for GCs might lie neither in universal phonetics nor in the set of contrasts within a lexical-phonological system, but in what Roman Jakobson has called the 'culminative' role of phonetic features. The following quotation by one of Jakobson's collaborators is probably relevant here.

In English, stress plays ... a cumulative rule in that it signals both the unity of the word and the number of words or word-groups in any given syntagm. In some languages, the device known as vowel harmony fills the similarly culminative rule of indicating the unity of the word. (Waugh 1987:163).

4.4 Some Practical Considerations

The historical phonology presented in this paper should be welcomed by researchers interested in pursuing further practical questions, such as: where did the Rejangs come from; how long have they been in Sumatra; which dialect represents the local homeland in Sumatra; what is the contact situation in Sumatra.

Preliminary answers are presented below in the form of four specific hypotheses guided by a general theory about a possible correlation between rate of linguistic change and distance traveled by out-migrating groups, proposed independently by Blust (1991b) and Ross (1991). The first concerns the problem of discovering an external subgroup and associated geographical point of origin for Rejang (4.4.); the second concerns the high number of innovations (over 100) separating PMP and any single contemporary Rejang dialect (4.4.3); the third posits the most likely 'local homeland' within Rejang country (4.4.2.2); and the last deals with the contemporary contact situation together with the question about which Austronesian group was the first to arrive in southern Sumatra.

4.4.1 The Search for an External Subgroup Smaller Than PMP

The discovery an external subgrouping hypothesis for Rejang may depend on three features that have been reconstructed for earlier stages of the language.

a) In pre-Rejang the accent fell regularly on the ultimate vowel when the penultimate vowel was schwa; otherwise on the penultimate (=Malay-type stress pattern).

b) In pre-Rejang PMP last-syllable *a underwent neutralization in two environments that can be generalized in terms of a single formula. See 4.2.2. The two neutralizations constitute the central problem of Rejang historical phonology as defined by McGinn (1997): they applied very early in the historical phonology (before the stress shifted to the contemporary pattern); and one of them (*-a(C) > *-«(C) except before velars) is typologically rare. In McGinn (2000, 2003) it is suggested that any language in the western Austronesian group that shared this pair of rules was eo ipso a candidate for subgrouping with Rejang. Such a language has indeed been reported in the literature by Christopher Court. Bukar-Sadong Land Dayak, spoken in the area around Serian, 3rd District Sarawak, Malaysian Borneo, betrays a similar (parallel or shared) history in two respects: PMP *-a regularly became schwa (PMP *duha > du«h 'two'); and PMP *-aC regularly became -«C except before velars (Court 1967). McGinn (2003) attempted but was ultimately forced to reject a direct subgrouping relationship between Bukar-Sadong Land Dayak and Rejang. At one and the same time, however, it was proposed that the early Rejangs probably migrated to Sumatra from someplace near the Land Dayak region of Borneo around 1200 years ago.

4.4.2 On Explaining High Rates of Linguistic Change

Blust (1991b) and Ross (1991) suggest there is a significant correlation between rate of sound change in a language and the geographical (migration) distance from the homeland. Let us call this the Blust-Ross Hypothesis (BRH). For example, Madagascar, off the coast of Africa, is inhabited by speakers of a closely-related dialects of a single language; Taiwan (a much smaller island off the coast of China) is inhabited by speakers of 22 highly diverse, distantly-related languages. Archeologists report that Madagascar has been occupied by Austronesian speakers for about 1,000 years; for Taiwan the figure is more like 6,000 years. Details aside, such facts are just what should be anticipated given the BRH.

An important fact about Rejang is the relatively high number of phonological changes: over 100 for any single dialect (Blust 1984; McGinn 1997, 2000, 2003). Considering just the four PMP vowels, Rejang has undergone more vocalic splits (27) than any other known Austronesian language. As mentioned earlier in this paper, however, dialect diversity among Rejang dialects is relatively slight: only Rawas shows divergence severe enough to impede mutual understanding: roughly 70-72% of basic vocabulary is shared between Rawas and each of the other four dialects. A high rate of change coupled with a low level of dialect diversity leads to a prediction: the Rejangs must have traveled to south-west Sumatra from a distant location relatively recently.

However, as pointed out by Robert Blust (personal communication), the prediction is not very compelling with respect to geography, particularly in light of arguments by McGinn (2003) that the Rejangs originated in Sarawak, Malaysian Borneo--a scant 600 miles away.

On the other hand, both Blust (1991b) and Ross (1991) have questioned whether geography is the crucial variable here; and Ross has offered an alternative. It can be added that Kiparsky (1988:383) believes that high rates of phonological change favor production, low rates favor perception. This bias is perhaps explained by Ross's alternative explanation --rejected by Blust--namely, that homeland languages are conservative because they are dominated by older speakers who tend to be intolerant of perceived mistakes in pronunciation and grammar. (In Kiparsky's terms, adult language-perceivers (hearers) predominate in the homeland.) By contrast, according to Ross, out-migrating language groups are dominated by younger adults with children, the latter being the major source of innovations. (In Kiparsky's terms, younger language-producers (speakers) predominate in out-migrating groups.) Combining these ideas, the crucial variable is probably not absolute geographical distance but 'psychological distance' (isolation) from the homeland.

Returning to the Rejang case, a plausible scenario is that the group migrated en masse without further contact, and without leaving behind sufficient population to maintain their cultural and linguistic identity in the homeland. If so, then Rejang's high number of innovations is consistent with its presumed geographical isolation from the original homeland (wherever it was).

4.4.3 Rawas As Local Homeland

The next question to ask is: which Rejang dialects are relatively more conservative, and which more innovative? Based on the BRH, conservative dialect(s) should point toward the local homeland, whereas the more innovative dialects should represent the communities that ventured out from there. As emphasized throughout this paper, Rawas is the most divergent dialect in phonology, grammar, and vocabulary--but is this caused by conservatism, innovation, or contact with Malay? The evidence suggests that all three factors have played a role, but that conservatism is the most prominent.

A telling argument for Rawas’ conservativism was mentioned in section 2: Every PR etymon can be reconstructed on the basis of just two dialects--either Rawas and Pesisir or (more often) Rawas and Kebanagung. The argument can be stated in another way as follows: It is impossible to derive the Rawas data from an alternative PR reconstructed on the basis of any two, three, or four of the remaining dialects. To repeat just one rather typical example: In all dialects except Rawas, word-final PR *-l from PMP *-l and *-R disappeared, and adjacent vowels PR *o and ä diphthongized to oa and ea respectively; but Rawas retained PR word-final *l and the adjacent vowels did not diphthongize. See 3.2.3. Readers can work out for themselves the impossibility of predicting Rawas biol 'water' given only PLMK bioa and PMP *wahiR.

If this argument is accepted together with the BRH, the conclusion is clear. The upper reaches of the Rawas River (Bioa Ab«s) represents the first area settled by the Rejangs in Sumatra.

4.4.4 Neighbors in Sumatra: Contact Issues

If Rawas, Kebanagung and Pesisir represent the 'outlier' dialect areas, the Lebong and Musi dialects occupy the Rejang heartland. They are centrally located; they are highest in elevation at the headwaters of the Ketaun and Musi rivers; and they occupy the political center (called Kabupaten Rejang-Lebong). There is even a working gold mine there. Importantly for purposes of this paper, the Lebong and Musi areas share no boundary with Malay-speaking populations: every point of contact with the world beyond Rejang-Lebong is either another Rejang dialect or uninhabited jungle. See map 1.1.

In this section are listed some linguistic elements in Rejang that appear to be the result of by areal pressures. The first two played important roles in the development of Rawas.

a) Borrowed -h obviated diphthongization of PR *-V? in Rawas and Kebanagung--the two dialects in closest contact with Malay. (See map 1.1.)

b) Borrowed PR *e (= mid front unrounded vowel) in the inventory of PR vowels contrasted with inherited PR from PMP *i, *a, *« via a number of regular vocalic shifts. However, the source (donor) language remains to be discovered.

c) Rejang's diphthongs--particularly the large number derived from PMP vowels (see Table 13)--might well receive an interpretation in terms of areal pressure. An important claim that bears repeating is that these diphthongs arose after dialect split in Rejang, hence represent late changes. It is highly likely that they developed in part in response to the larger social milieu which included linguistic contact (intermarriage, etc.) with at least two neighboring languages likewise displaying large numbers of diphthongs, namely, Kerinci (Prentice and Hakim 1978; Blust 1984:440) and Minangkabau Malay.

d) As pointed out in 3.3.2, three Rejang pronouns are unexplained: PLMK nu (expected mu**); PLMK udi '2Ppl'; and Rawas kab«n '2Psg' expected kaw**). Possibly Rawas kab«n is an internal borrowing based on kab«n 'friend' (cf. Ujan Mas Malay kaba (McGinn 1991:219); and PLMK nu '2Psg' may be borrowed from Lampung niku.

Blust (1992) has argued that Malayic speakers were relatively late arrivals in Sumatra. If so, the Rejangs likely preceded them. This view is consistent both with linguistic facts and with legends on both sides. Rejangs refer to themselves as tun asl«y ‘original people’, and the Besemah Malays appear to agree. According to William Collins (1998), some Besemah megaliths are called makam Rejang (`Rejang graves'), and Besemah legends explain how the Rejangs were displaced by Besemah founder Atung Bungsu by means of a cleverly worded oath. Whatever the causes, Rejang farmers came to occupy the highlands whilst Malay farmers took the surrounding lowlands; and several other major language groups (the Komering, the Kerinci, and the Lampungese) found their way into the country as well. Meanwhile, the Malay-speaking Javano-Malay empire of Sriwijaya rose and fell in Palembang, the unrivaled center of prestige in the region (Coedes1992).

5. Possible Alternatives for Proto-Rejang Vowels and Diphthongs

This section presents some alternatives for deriving the Rawas vowels and diphthongs from Proto-Rejang and ultimately PMP. The alternatives are presented as less plausible than the analyses that appeared in the body of the paper. The discussion will be guided by two principles which serve to impose limits on linguistic reconstructions.

Realism: Protolanguages must conform to the expectations of attested languages. This is a theoretical (a priori) condition marking as highly suspicious the reconstruction of phonemes or arrays of phonemes that are not to be found in attested languages anywhere on earth. Moreover, since grammars tend overwhelmingly to display motivated structures, it follows that protolanguages should be the same. My claims that the PR vowel inventory consisted of seven vowels in an ordered array, and that the PR lexicon was governed by vowel harmony, represent two proposals for a structured protolanguage named PR. A protolanguage failing to yield plausible structures fails to be realistic.

Uniformitarianism: Protolanguages must be motivated by the evidence of the set of languages and dialects presumably derived from them. In this respect, PR is a plausible reconstruction because the Rawas dialect bears direct witness to it. By contrast, PR *-j from PMP *-j is not supported by -j in any of the dialects but rather by a phonetically ambivalent formal correspondence, namely, PMP/PR *-j > k = g = t. In fact, PMP *-j is puzzling phonetically and may always remain problematic (Blust 1991c:132) .[27] It has proven its usefulness in Rejang Historical Phonology (and repeated for other Austronesian language groups) as a way of capturing the formal regularity of a phonetically disjunctive correspondence set. But it remains problematic from the standpoint of the uniformitarian principle.

Four interesting cases arise in RHP showing how the two principles mention above can come into conflict. Two have to do with the seven-vowel system reconstructed for PR; the remaining two have to do with diphthongs derived from PMP vowels.

1) Alternatives for deriving two Rawas diphthongs from PMP/PR vowels:

a) Why not replace PR *-i with *-« from PMP *-a (e.g. *mat« from PMP *mata ‘eye’)

at the level of Proto-Rejang? (Recall that *-« represents an essential

intermediate step in the derivation of Rawas «y from PMP *-a.)

b) Why not reconstruct diphthongized end-rhymes PR -*ia? and *-ua? to underlie

Pesisir and Lebong -ia? and -ua? corresponding to Rawas -äh and -oh?

(Recall that in the body of the paper these diphthongs were derived from PR *-i?

and *-u?, respectively.)

2) Alternatives to the seven-vowel inventory of vowels for Proto-Rejang:

c) Why not reconstruct PR * alongside to yield an eight-vowel system for PR?

d) Why not remove PR and thereby posit a six-vowel inventory for Proto-Rejang?

These four alternatives are considered in turn below.

a) PR *mati: ‘eye’. Why not reconstruct PR *mat«: from PMP *mata ‘eye’ parallel to PR *it« from PMP *(k)ita ‘1Ppl(incl)’? As pointed out in section 4.2.2, Rawas mat«y ‘eye’ represents the regular outcome based on a sequence of rules which includes *mat«: (with stress on the ultimate vowel) as intermediate form. But *mat«: was pre-Rejang; the proposed PR form was *mati: which directly underlies Rawas mat«y. The full derivation is: PMP *mata > ma:ta > *ma:t« > *mat«: > PR *mati: > Rawas mat«y ‘eye’. It must be admitted that reconstructing *mat«: instead of *mati: at the level of PR would be highly attractive from the realist perspective, because *mat«: conforms to the vowel harmony structures posited for PR (unlike *mati: which breaks the mold). However, realism does not imply perfection; real languages are often imperfect. After all, structures do undergo change; moreover, *mati: represents the prelude to several diphthongization processes that 'conspired' to (partially) destroy vowel harmony.

The ultimate reason to prefer PR *mati: over *mat«: is that the former involves less abstraction. In every dialect PR stressed *-i: from PMP *-a developed exactly the same as PR stressed *-i: from PMP *-i. Put in another way, *mati: represents the ‘safer’ alternative from the uniformitarian perspective.

b) PR *pulu? ‘ten and PR *ili? ‘choose’. Why not reconstruct PR *pulua? from PMP *puluq ‘ten’ and PR *iliä? from PMP *piliq ‘choose’? This may be a question for phoneticians, for what is at stake is the derivation of Rawas äläh from either PR *ili? or *ilia?. Given PR *ili? we have a longer derivation that includes diphthongization: *ili? > *ilih > *iliäh > *iläh > äläh. Given PR *ilia? the derivation is one step shorter: *iliä? > *iliäh > *iläh > äläh. Actually, there is little to choose between these two alternatives considered by themselves. However, the parallel case of PMP ‘ten’ is not quite so ambivalent. PR *pulu? yields the simpler derivation: *pulu? > *puluh > *puloh > *poloh. By contrast, PR *pulua? would require not only a longer derivation, but also a rather dubious reversal of simple vowel to diphthong and back again to simple vowel: PMP *puluq > *pulua? > *puluah > *puloah > poloh. A strict uniformitarian would perhaps argue that Rawas poloh shows no evidence of diphthongization, so why impose it on the derivation? A structural advantage is also gained by adopting this perspective here: it supports a potentially significant generalization, namely, all contemporary diphthongs from PMP vowels developed after dialect split.

Next, we consider the consequences of replacing PR's seven-vowel inventory (based on Rawas) with either an eight-vowel inventory (witnessed by none of the dialects) or a six-vowel inventory (witnessed by four of five dialects). In either alternative, the historical status of Rawas ä is the focus of attention.

c) Why not reconstruct PR * parallel to ? As readers can easily work out for themselves, it is certainly plausible to propose a symmetrical eight-vowel system for PR, and also a set of diachronic rules with greater parallelism than the analysis presented in the body of the paper. For example, alongside regular changes like PMP *laNit > PR *läNät ‘sky’ there would be PMP *manuk > *mnk ‘chicken’ and an extra rule such that PR * > o was an unconditioned pan-dialectal rule paralleling the (absolutely necessary) unconditioned rule PR > e that affected all dialects except Rawas. Notice that this alternative eight-vowel system is perfectly reasonable from the realist point of view, and may even be correct. But if so, it must overcome the uniformitarian objection that the proposed contrast between PR * and *o is not supported by any of the contemporary dialects. Strong arguments (not ventured here) would be required to overturn the uniformitarian objection in this case.

d) Why not eliminate PR and posit six-vowels for PR based on PLMK? The answer offered here is somewhat tentative. A six vowel system (*i, *u, *«, *e, *o, *a) is supported by contemporary PLMK. Given such a system for PR, Rawas ä would have developed after dialect split. One consequence is that the correspondence set e-e = e-e = e-e = e-e = ä-ä would have to reflect PR vowel-pair *e-e; moreover , a number of changes would be needed to account for the Rawas ä in these examples and several other types of cases. Such an analysis would work if every PR *e were derivable from PMP, but that is apparently not the case. Consider the fact that PR *kidek became Rawas kedek and not kädäk** or kidäk**. The assumption that *e and contrasted in PR (as in contemporary Rawas) explains why contemporary Rawas ä always reflects vowels inherited from PMP, whereas Rawas e never does. The explanation is straightforward on the assumption that Rawas is conservative, and continues to reflect the PR distinction between inherited phonemes (*ä-ä) derived directly from PMP vowels, and borrowed phonemes e and e-e which did not exist in PMP and which, after entering pre-Rejang, remained distinct in PR, as in Rawas, while in the other dialects (inherited) and (borrowed) *e merged as e. On these assumptions, it is reasonable to claim that PMP *laNit became PR *läNät 'sky' alongside borrowed PR *kidek, and then, after dialect split, the following two changes occurred. (i) In PLMK PR *läNät became leNet via unconditioned change > e. (ii) In Rawas *kidek > kede?. It just seems harder (perhaps impossible) to justify reconstructing PMP *laNit to become PR *leNet alongside PR *kidek, and then deriving the Rawas outcomes. How could putative PR *leNet become Nät via the change *e > ä while at the same time *kidek became kedek and not kädäk** or at least kidäk**?

A second objection to the six-vowel hypothesis concerns the phonetic motivation for outcomes like Rawas äpän = PLMK epen from PMP *ipen [ip«n]. In a PR six-vowel system, PR *epen is perfectly harmonized, which leaves little room to motivate the change *e > ä needed in Rawas to produce äpän. The only possible motivation would be a theoretically highly dubious one, namely, a 'push-chain' effect whereby non-native words like kedek 'bad' (source unknown), sen 'money' (Dutch), and laher 'born' (Malay) had to be distinguished from native words like putative PR *epen by changing them to äpän. Since the days of Rask and Grimm, theories of linguistic change have sought to explain sound shifts on phonetic or (more recently) phonological grounds. Clearly such grounds are lacking in this account.

Therefore, if our analysis is accepted, PR had a seven-vowel system (including ). It follows that after dialect split, all dialects except Rawas underwent unconditioned change PR > e, thereby broadening the distribution of PR *e. By contrast, in Rawas there were four types of vocalic changes which broadened the distribution of ä. First, vowel coalescence produced Rawas ä from the PR sequence *-ai-, e.g. Rawas ? from PR *naik (*nahik 'climb'), and Rawas pät from PR *pait (< style="">*paqit 'bitter'). Second, the Rawas word-final rhyme -äh as in putäh 'white' and äläh 'choose' regularly reflects PR *-i? from PMP *-iq (PMP *putiq 'white' and PMP *piliq 'choose'). Third, the derivation of Rawas äläh = PLMK from PR *ili? (< style="">*piliq 'choose') shows one case of the vowel-pair ä-ä developing after dialect split in Rawas (whereas Nät 'sky' developed before split). Finally, the pronoun kämäy shows another case. Presumably PR *kimi (from PMP *kami '1Ppl(excl)') by diphthongization (> *kim«y) followed by an unexplained harmonization change modeled on an earlier perfectly regular change (PMP and PR *ip«n > äpän ‘tooth’).

So far our survey of sources of Rawas ä and e has turned up e only from borrowed sources, whereas ä occurs in both borrowed words and inherited words.[28] This general situation is best explained in terms of three assumptions adopted throughout this paper: (a) PR contrasted with PR *e; (b) the distribution of ä from PR expanded in Rawas; and (c) the distribution e from PR *e expanded in the other dialects through an unconditioned change of PR > e (see set A, table 12).

I should like to conclude this paper with a quotation from Nature writer Rebecca L. Cann (2000).

Words do not fossilize. Yet they leave evidence of their evolution in the populations that speak them, in much the same way that genes reveal the evolutionary history of the populations that transmit them.

A standard assumption in historical phonology, well-supported by the evidence of this paper, is that dialect differences develop from (mostly) regular changes that may affect each dialect slightly differently (Blust 1991c, 1999). By following standard techniques of linguistic reconstruction, aspects of the linguistic history of a set of dialects (such as the five Rejang dialects under investigation here) can sometimes be reconstructed, and aspects of the past thereby revealed. In addition, lexicostatistical and glottochronological techniques, although admittedly crude and inexact, may allow such results to be combined with evidence from other fields (such as archeology and genetics) to be mapped onto a graph representing years of separation (Bellwood, Fox and Tryon 1995). The evidence thus extracted from various fields will someday provide the necessary facts and arguments for understanding the external history of the Rejangs: where they came from, how long they have occupied the Barisan highlands of southwest Sumatra, and whether they preceded or followed the other language groups presently occupying the surrounding lowlands. Consideration of such questions has been touched upon in this paper only in terms of possible consequences that may follow reasonably from the linguistic evidence. But the answers provided here represent only the first, somewhat tentative, proposals in relation to these broader issues. At one and the same time, an important goal will have been met if the questions raised in this paper serve to encourage future research in linguistics, archeology, anthropology, history, and education relating to the Rejang people and their Sumatran neighbors.



[1] I would like to express my appreciation to the Rejang speakers who offered information about their dialects: Ismail Amir (Kebanagung), Arma Zuazla and Sahril Umar (Musi), Pak Anwar (Padang Bendar, Pesisir); Ibu Baima (Embong Panjang, Lebong); Mo. Haji Daud, Pak Ibraham, Mariam, and Kartila S.E. (Muara Kulam, Rawas). I also wish to thank Dr. Zainubi Arbi of Kepahiyang and Pak Sabidin Ishak of Curup for invaluable assistance spanning thirty years. Likewise I am grateful to Dr. Amran Halim, Dr. Zainab Bakir and Dr. Chuzaimah Diem, all at the University of Srivijaya, Palembang, for many kindnesses and much help with facilities and resources. Finally, I wish to thank Dr. Robert A. Blust for reading an earlier draft of this article and making many helpful suggestions. All errors of fact and interpretation remain mine alone.

[2] Abbreviations and special symbols used in the paper are as follows:

colon (:) = accented vowel on the word-level, e.g. V: vs. V (unaccented)

end-rhyme = -V# or -VC#

n.c. = non-cognate

n.d. = no data

dialect abbreviations: P = Pesisir; L = Lebong; M = Musi; K = Kebanagung; R = Rawas; PL = Pesisir and Lebong; PLM = Pesisir, Lebong and Musi, etc.

[3] Kebanagung -/k/ provided crucial evidence explaining an apparently irregular change affecting kin terms (McGinn 1997:68).

[4] Blust (1991c:132) describes PMP *j as a voiced palatalized velar stop that occurred word-finally and between vowels.

[5] One exception is Rawas /belo?/ ‘turn’ from Malay belok (expected bele?**). Note that the expected form would not, in Rawas, be homophonous with inherited /bälä?/ from PMP *balik ‘return’.

[6] Morphological variants reflecting PMP *piliq and PR *ili? include Pesisir: /milia?/ (active) ~ /nilia?/ (passive) ~/kilia?/ (imperative) ~/pilia?/ (nominal).

[7] It is noteworthy that all words beginning /b«m…/ or /p«m…/, although historically probably infixed with -/«m/- 'active', have undergone reanalysis into two prefixes, e.g. Musi /b«monoa?/ / ‘die off’ = {b«-} + {m(«)-} + {onoa?}.

[8] Many Rawas words with final -/l/ derive from Malay or from unknown sources:

Rawas PLMK Malay Gloss

tiNal (syn. /di«m/) tiNa tinggal wait, stay, live

batal bata bantal pillow

m«sol m«soa cari look for; hunt

tokol palu hammer

nugal to dibble; plant by dibbling

macol buko buka to open; take off (clothes)

tukäl tukil bamboo wine-making instrument

kacäl kacea kancil mouse-deer

cukäl kikoa gali dig

[9]A discrepancy is hereby noted between my data and Blust's (1984:427) with respect to the Musi words for 'finger' and 'tapering'. My data shows Musi /ji?«y/ and /ti?us/ with the expected regular development of -/?/- from PMP *-r-, whereas Blust recorded /ji«y/ and /tius/.

[10] Given that PMP *Z and *z have collapsed into a single phoneme *z in recent literature (Blust 1999), the GC shown here is needed to preserve regularity for the Rejang outcomes.

[11] Schwa Syncope remains an active (synchronic) rule applying across morpheme boundaries in contemporary Rejang, e.g. Musi {-«m-} 'active' + {t«Noa} 'hear' --> /t«mNoa/ 'to hear'.

[12] PR *-t from PMP *-j is irregular (expected PR -j).

[13] In early pre-Rejang the accent fell on the final syllabic when the penult was schwa; otherwise on the penult. See McGinn (1997, 2000).

[14] Blust (1982) describes a similar process of syllable reduction in the history of Malay.

[15] PMP *-e (schwa) did not occur word-finally.

[16] Rawas /belok/ is borrowed from Malay belok 'turn'.

[17] Table 10 is adapted from McGinn (1997, Table 14) with Rawas data added.

[18] The Rejang place-name Lebong [l«boN] is potentially interesting in this context. If from PMP *lebeN (presumably pronounced [l«b«N]) 'valley' then Lebong [l«boN] is regular.

[19] Rawas pokot and tokot are exceptions (Table 12).

[20] Blust (1984:434) assumes that Musi /puk«t/ was borrowed from Malay pukat.

[21] The alternation is by no means grammaticalized; each pronoun can fulfill either function as governed by discourse rules.

[22] In analyzing Rejang diphthongs McGinn (1997) followed Blust (1984) and not McGinn (1983).

[23] This topic is explored in McGinn (2003) in connection with the search for a subgrouping hypothesis.

[24] The form of the argument may be schematized as follows, where 1,2,3 are sound changes. Situation A: 1. a > b / c__d; 2. b > e / c__d; 3. a does not become e / c__d; therefore, rule 2 preceded rule 1. Situation B: 1. a > b /c__d; b > e /f__g; therefore, the ordering relation between rules 1 and 2 is indeterminate.

[25] In focus here is not the deeper question of how to define a diphthong (that would take us too far afield), but rather, how the traditional definition (=a phoneme consisting of a syllabic vowel and a non-syllabic vowel) interacts with stress in the historical phonology of a language.

[26] James W. Harris (1985:31) stated the following about Spanish: “Essentially, the paradox is that the rules of stress and diphthongization must each refer to the output of the other.”

[27] “(PMP) *j was a palatalized velar stop [gy] ... it had no voiceless counterpart; ...(it was) an ‘island’ within the phoneme inventory.”

[28] Rawas /ät/ 'dirty; worn out' is from an unknown source (cf. Ml jahat), as are /käkäl/ 'foot' (cf. Malay kaki) and /kacäl/ 'mouse-deer' (cf. Malay kancil).

0 comments:

Rejang Land Pal

Support by

Add to Technorati Favorites blog-indonesia.com